Skip to main content

State or trait: the neurobiology of anorexia nervosa — contributions of a functional magnetic resonance imaging study

Abstract

Background

The understanding of the cerebral neurobiology of anorexia nervosa (AN) with respect to state- versus trait-related abnormalities is limited. There is evidence of restitution of structural brain alterations with clinical remission. However, with regard to functional brain abnormalities, this issue has not yet been clarified.

Methods

We compared women with AN (n = 31), well-recovered female participants (REC) (n = 18) and non-patients (NP) (n = 27) cross-sectionally. Functional magnetic resonance imaging was performed to compare neural responses to food versus non-food images. Additionally, affective ratings were assessed.

Results

Functional responses and affective ratings did not differ between REC and NP, even when applying lenient thresholds for the comparison of neural responses. Comparing REC and AN, the latter showed lower valence and higher arousal ratings for food stimuli, and neural responses differed with lenient thresholds in an occipital region.

Conclusions

The data are in line with some previous findings and suggest restitution of cerebral function with clinical recovery. Furthermore, affective ratings did not differ from NP. These results need to be verified in intra-individual longitudinal studies.

Plain English summary

There is abundant evidence of structural and functional brain alterations during the acute stage of anorexia nervosa (AN), although affected brain areas differ based on various study methodologies. Meanwhile, investigations indicate that brain structure normalizes with weight and clinical restitution. The current cross-sectional investigation examines acutely ill AN patients, healthy controls, i.e. non-patients (NP) and well-recovered individuals (REC), with respect to brain function. Functional cerebral responses of participants exposed to food pictures were investigated. Neither in terms of function nor emotional experience of food stimuli, the REC differed from the NP group. This study points to brain function normalizing with clinical and weight restoration, which should be verified in intra-individual longitudinal studies.

Background

Anorexia nervosa (AN) is an eating disorder largely affecting young women with high morbidity, chronicity and mortality [1]. Apart from a restriction of energy intake leading to a significant weight loss, an intense fear of gaining weight and body image disturbance are key symptoms. The etiology is not well understood, though genetic disposition is one important factor, accompanied by psychosocial factors [2]. Maintaining factors include consequences of malnutrition, as well as consequences of isolation and depression [3]. Brain imaging studies point towards shrinkage of white (WM) and grey matter (GM) and complementary increase of the cerebrospinal spaces [4, 5]. Longitudinal investigations show restitution of GM and WM volumetric alterations following long-term weight restoration [4]. Apart from structural brain alterations, also the function of various domains has been reported to be affected in acute AN [6, 7], although the involved brain areas differed [8]. Due to significant loss of weight, cerebral aberrations might be associated with metabolic changes, i.e. starvation, and it is difficult to disentangle other factors associated with eating disorder psychopathology and/or predisposing factors [9].

Hence, the question remains which abnormalities are state phenomena, i.e. occur only during the acute phase of the disease, and which are trait-related. Persistent cerebral aberrations could be a predisposing phenomenon and might therefore represent endophenotypes [10], but they may also represent sequels, i.e. “scars” of the acute disease. As AN is a disorder with low prevalence [2], it is methodically difficult to study individuals who might develop AN longitudinally in order to shed light on the question of endophenotypic cerebral aberrations. Therefore, a first step to clarify questions about state and trait is to investigate individuals recovered from AN (REC).

With respect to the issue of state-trait in AN, previous studies examined psychological, cognitive and behavioral variables [11, 12] as well as metabolic, structural and functional correlates of the brain [13,14,15]. Overall, research on reversibility of psychological, cognitive and behavioral functioning in REC presents a heterogeneous picture [16,17,18]. In terms of structural cerebral alterations, neuroimaging studies provide strong evidence of remission with clinical recovery [5, 19, 20]. Findings of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies comparing REC and non-patients (NP) show divergent results. With respect to visual food cues, some authors reported no alteration in brain activation in REC (when compared to NP) [21,22,23], while Uher et al. [24] detected increased activation of prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices (ACC) and a reduction of activity in parietal regions in a small group of REC. Further studies found hypoactivation of the insula [25] or increased caudate activation [26] in response to food pictures. With regard to other disorder specific paradigms (e.g. taste, body shape), both increased and decreased functional neural brain responses for REC (compared to NP) were found in various brain regions [13, 27,28,29,30], while two studies detected no altered brain activity in REC [31, 32]. However, a majority of fMRI studies using non-disorder specific stimuli (e.g., fear, intimacy, reward) reported no or only minimal functional aberrations in REC [33,34,35,36]. Longitudinal fMRI studies with non-disorder specific paradigms yielded conflicting results in delay discounting tasks [37, 38], a normalization in a working memory and set-shifting task [39, 40], and persisting changes in theory of mind and reward learning paradigms [41, 42]. For further details and an overview of previous studies, see Additional file 1.

This investigation focuses on the question of restitution vs. non-restitution of functional brain abnormalities using a cross-sectional design in order to address the topic state and trait of the neurobiology in AN. In this context, the comparison of NP and REC is of particular interest. It complements previous studies on disease-specific food stimuli in REC [21,22,23,24,25,26]. The paradigm has already been employed previously in a study of AN with some REC participants [24, 43] and a study focusing on NP versus AN [44]. Data from the current study comparing NP and AN has been reported previously, with a focus on replicability issues [45]: Group comparisons yielded higher blood oxygenation-level dependent (BOLD) responses of AN compared to NP in midcingulate, pre/postcentral and parietal areas when using a lenient initial threshold, and no significant group differences with a conservative initial threshold.

Based on results of preceding whole-brain analyses [23, 25], we expected no differences between REC and NP in neural response to food stimuli or behavioral/experiential response, i.e., affective ratings of stimuli [22, 24, 46]. Additionally, we performed exploratory analyses of affective ratings and insula activation, as earlier studies found a positive correlation between food pleasantness ratings and insula activation in the NP group [22, 25], but not in the AN [25] or REC groups [22].

Methods

Participants

AN and REC participants were recruited via the Department of Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy of the University Medical Center Freiburg. NP were recruited via local advertisements. The study was performed following written informed consent from the participants. The data was collected between March 2015 and October 2017.

Thirty-one AN, 18 REC and 27 NP were included in the final analysis. AN participants had to fulfill DSM-5 criteria. The following inclusion criteria were defined for the REC group: (1) Absence of eating disorder symptomatology for more than 12 months and an Eating Disorder Examination (EDE) [47] within one standard deviation of normal; (2) The Body Mass Index (BMI) was aimed at ≥ 20 kg/m2, which we achieved for most REC. The BMI of four participants was slightly below 20 kg/m2 (between 19.3 and 19.7 kg/m2) and of one participant 18.8 kg/m2. We decided to include these participants because they were clinically completely recovered and had always had a BMI in this range before the onset of the disease. Three AN patients were of the binge eating/purging type, all other AN and REC were of the restrictive type. Patients with AN were seen in the outpatient clinic for diagnostic reasons while nine were right at the beginning of inpatient treatment. Exclusion criteria had been reported previously [45].

The participants examined in the current study largely overlap with those of our previous investigations [5, 14, 19, 34, 35, 45].

Procedure

The study was approved by the local Ethics Committee (EK 520/13). Participants were assessed by means of the SCID interview [48, 49], the EDE [47, 50] and the following self-report questionnaires: Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) [51], Eating Disorder Inventory-2 (EDI-2) [52], State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) [53], and a crystalline intelligence test (MWT-B) [54]. All participants were studied in the second half of the menstrual cycle or the corresponding phase with estrogen and progesterone when taking oral contraception. In the morning around 8 a.m., participants were provided with a standardized breakfast, the calories consumed were counted, and the feeling of satiety was rated on a Likert-scale from 0 (very hungry) to 9 (very satiated).

Paradigm

Participants viewed via a mirror photographs of food and non-food items of similar structure [43,44,45] presented on a BOLDScreen monitor at the rear of the scanner bore. In a block design with five blocks per condition of 30 s each, 10 consecutive food or non-food pictures were presented alternately per block.

Participants were asked to look attentively at the pictures. Examples of the picture stimuli are displayed in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Examples of food and non-food stimuli (cf. [45])

Behavioral data

After scanning, participants rated a selection of images (10 food and 10 non-food images) with respect to three emotion dimensions (valence, arousal and dominance) using “manikin ratings” based on the International Affective Picture System on scales from zero to eight [54]. The dominance scale assesses how much the viewer feels controlled or in control when watching the images [55]. Ratings of two AN and two NP could not be included due to incomplete data.

Image acquisition and processing

A T1-weighted Magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo (MPRAGE) sequence (TR = 2300 ms, TE = 2.98 ms, flip angle = 9°, FOV = 240 * 256 mm2, voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm3) was recorded as an anatomical reference. 138 functional echo-planar T2*-weighted (EPI) images (TR = 2500 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, FOV = 192 * 192 mm2, Matrix volume = 64 × 64, 38 slices, voxel size = 3 × 3 × 3 mm3) were recorded with a Siemens 3 T Prisma MAGNETOM (Siemens Medical Systems, Erlangen) using a 20-channel head coil. All EPI images were automatically rigid-body transformed to correct for head motion and a distortion correction algorithm was applied [56]. Preprocessing and statistical analysis of the functional data was performed with the statistical parametric mapping software SPM12 (Wellcome Trust Centre of Imaging Neuroscience, London; for details, see [57]). The first two volumes of each run were disregarded and an artifact detection algorithm (ArtRepair toolbox, SPM) was applied to detect head motion and possible spiking artifacts. The functional raw images were realigned to the first volume to generate six head motion parameters (rotation and translation in x, y, z direction), which were used as regressors of no interest in the first-level statistical analysis to correct for influences of head motion. The ‘Artrepair’ tool implemented in SPM12 was used to correct movement artifacts over half a voxel size by interpolating the measurement time points before and after the movement. Participants whose head movements were larger than half a voxel size (corresponding to 1.5 mm for a voxel size of 3 × 3 × 3 mm3) were excluded from the analysis if more than two consecutive measurement time points (= "volumes") were affected or more than two corrections had to be made in the time series. In the case of spiking artifacts again the ‘Artrepair’ toolbox was used to correct single slices by interpolating the slice below and above the affected slice. If several slices of a single volume were affected, we interpolated (correspondingly to motion artifacts) the measurement time points before and after the volume affected by spiking artifacts. If two consecutive volumes or more than two volumes in total were affected by spiking artifacts, the subject was excluded. The motion corrected images were spatially normalized into the MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) reference system applying the anatomical MPRAGE image. To increase the signal-to-noise ratio and to compensate for inter-individual differences in location of corresponding functional areas, the data was spatially smoothed with a three-dimensional isotropic Gaussian kernel (8 mm FWHM). Low frequency artifacts across the time-series were removed applying a high-pass filter (128 s).

Statistical analysis

Demographic, clinical and behavioral data were assessed by analyses of variance with a level of significance of p < 0.05 (two-sided).

Food and non-food regressors were convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function and fitted together with the six regressors for head motion parameters in a linear regression model (general linear model (GLM)) with the functional signal time courses for each voxel and participant.

Within-group activation

In the second-level whole brain analysis, we tested for within-group differences (group activation) by performing a one-sample t-test for the food > non-food contrast of the first-level beta estimates of the food and non-food regressors.

Between-group comparison

For group comparisons, the first-level food > non-food contrast was used to compare AN > REC, REC > AN, NP > REC, and REC > NP in a two-sample t-test.

For both, the within- and between-group analysis: (1) We added age as a covariate. (2) We performed whole brain analyses with a cluster-defining threshold, i.e., initial height threshold, of puncorr. < 0.001 and a minimum cluster size of 10 voxels (k ≥ 10) (3) Results were corrected for multiple comparisons on a cluster level applying family-wise error correction with a threshold of pcorr. < 0.05.

For the between-group analysis, we additionally performed analyses with a cluster-defining threshold of puncorr. < 0.01 and a minimum cluster size of 10 voxels, as previous studies had used lenient thresholds of puncorr. < 0.01 [24, 44] or even lower (puncorr. < 0.05) [22, 25].

Moreover, we performed ROI-based (region of interest) small volume correction (SVC) for insula and amygdala ROIs according to the AAL3 atlas [58].

Comparisons of the AN and NP group are not reported as they have already previously been published [45].

Multiple regression analysis

For all groups and for each group separately, SPM multiple regression analyses were performed to calculate the correlation of the first-level BOLD contrast (food and non-food) with the valence and arousal ratings of the food stimuli. We tested for positive and negative linear effects of stimulus ratings and included the factor group as a regressor of non-interest (to adjust for possible group differences). We set up an interaction model with the factors group and stimulus ratings as regressors of interest and age as regressor of non-interest and compared the regression slopes of AN > NP, AN > REC, and NP > REC and vice versa.

Behavioral data

We set-up three separate Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) models for valence/arousal/dominance (dependent variable) with the independent factor group followed by post-hoc Tukey Kramer tests to assess for between-group differences.

Results

Participants

One hundred and eight female participants (40 AN, 24 REC, 44 NP) were recruited. Thirty-two data sets had to be discarded due to insufficient data quality, spiking head motion or incomplete data (Fig. 2). The food paradigm was performed towards the end of the imaging session (after approximately 35 min). This might have resulted in increased head motion (three subjects) and termination of the session by the participants (five). Spiking artifacts were also more likely to occur at the end of a scanning session, possibly due to the scanner heating up, requiring the exclusion of further 22 participants (Fig. 2). Seventy-six functional data sets could finally be analyzed: 31 AN, 18 REC and 27 NP.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Exclusion flow chart. AN anorexia nervosa, NP non-patients, REC recovered AN

Post-hoc power calculation

A post-hoc power calculation for the food > non-food contrast with Random Field Theory control was performed separately for the final AN (power 0.81 with cluster-defining threshold p < 0.001, power 0.59 with cluster-defining threshold p < 0.01), REC (power 0.32 with cluster-defining threshold p < 0.001, power 0.24 with cluster-defining threshold p < 0.01) and NP (power 0.88 with cluster-defining threshold p < 0.001, power 0.54 with cluster-defining threshold p < 0.01) samples applying the “Neuro-Powertool” (http://neuropowertools.org/; retrieval date 05th May, 2022).

Demographic and clinical characteristics

Patients with AN had typical features of psychopathology and a lower BMI compared to the REC and NP group. REC participants showed good restitution of all clinical data (Table 1). The NP group was matched to the AN group and therefore younger than REC (Table 1). Lowest BMI and duration of illness did not differ between REC and AN. Patients with AN consumed fewer calories than REC and NP, but did not differ in feeling of satiety.

Table 1 Demographic and clinical characteristics

Within-group activation

The contrast food > non-food showed increased BOLD activation of frontoinsular cortices in all three groups (Table 2, Fig. 3a). Within-group analyses showed no significant results in any of the three groups for the contrast non-food > food.

Table 2 Within-group differences for the contrast food > non-food (cluster-defining threshold of puncorr. < 0.001, k ≥ 10 voxels)
Fig. 3
figure 3

A T-maps of within-group differences for the contrast food > non-food for AN, REC and NP, puncorr. < 0.001, k ≥ 10 voxels. Color bars represent the t-scores (white/yellow = high, red = low). B Behavioral data. Mean valence (0 = negative to 8 = positive valence), arousal (0 = unaroused to 8 = very aroused) and dominance (0 = not dominant to 8 = very dominant) ratings for the three groups (AN, REC, NP) and the two conditions (food and non-food). *p < 0.01, **p < 0.001. AN anorexia nervosa; NP non-patients, REC recovered AN (cf. [45])

Between-group comparison

Applying the initial threshold of puncorr. < 0.001, the two-sample t-test revealed no significant BOLD differences between groups for any of the comparisons (AN > REC, REC > AN, NP > REC, REC > NP).

At a lenient cluster-forming initial threshold of puncorr. < 0.01, there were no differences between REC and NP neither. AN showed a higher BOLD response of an occipital area compared to REC (Additional file 2). Comparisons of AN and NP have been reported previously [45], where AN showed higher BOLD responses compared to NP in midcingulate, pre/postcentral and parietal areas when using a lenient initial threshold, and no significant group differences with recommended more stringent initial cluster-forming thresholds [45, 59]. Neither the SVC analysis for the insula nor for the amygdala ROIs survived family-wise error correction.

Multiple regression analysis

None of the regression models yielded results that exceeded the significance threshold. Additional file 3 depicts the results of the performed multiple regression models for the affective ratings (arousal/valence).

Behavioral data

The AN group had lower valence and higher arousal ratings compared to both REC and NP, while REC did not differ from NP (Table 3; Fig. 3b). There were no group differences in the ratings of non-food stimuli.

Table 3 Behavioral data

Discussion

This cross-sectional fMRI study on functional neural correlates towards disease-related stimuli (food vs. non-food images) revealed no group differences between REC and NP with neither a conservative initial height threshold [45] nor a lenient one. REC and NP also showed no difference in affective ratings, i.e. with respect to the subjective experience of the stimuli. Furthermore, there was no significant correlation of the valence and arousal ratings with the BOLD response towards food stimuli in the insular cortex.

The lack of group differences between REC and NP in functional neural correlates to visual food cues is in line with results of previous studies, which also detected no differences between REC and NP [21,22,23]. However, some studies reported differences between groups [24,25,26], which was only the case in region-of-interest analyses, but not whole-brain in the Holsen et al. sample [25]. These inconsistencies are likely due to a variety of reasons, some of which are associated with limited reliability and replicability [45]. Further, it is likely that studies yielded false-positive results due to small sample sizes as well as different statistical methods (e.g., region of interest vs. whole brain analyses, statistical thresholds, etc.) [60, 61]. Comparability of studies is hampered due to heterogeneity across participants (BMI, duration of illness and recovery, etc.), within participants (daytime, hormonal level, etc.), and across study sites (study design, scanner hardware, etc.) [45, 62]. This, once again, supports the need of replication studies [63], especially in the field of fMRI [64, 65]. With respect to the early study of Uher et al. [24], which reported group differences of functional neural correlates and which used visual stimuli similar to ours, these factors likely account for the differing results.

Naturally, recovery criteria are another important topic with respect to state and trait. Recovery in AN in principle requires the absence of eating disorder psychopathology and a minimal BMI for a certain period of time [66,67,68,69], although the exact quantitative values vary.

For further detailed discussion of this topic with respect to recovery, remission and relapse see for example Khalsa et al. [69]. We used conservative measures in the current study, i.e. absence of eating disorder pathology for at least 12 months, an EDE score within one standard deviation of normal. Most REC were in the BMI ≥ 20 kg/m2 range with a few exceptions. In consequence, our sample of REC represents clinically stable long-term recovered individuals, which seems important in order to avoid cerebral aberrations and dysfunction—which might still be present after short term—due to insufficient clinical recovery.

However, most previous studies used such recovery criteria (see Additional file 1), and we could not detect any pattern with respect to the rather divergent study results.

We aimed to put behavioral and cerebral response to visual food cues in the context of state and trait. In contrast to AN, REC participants did not differ psychometrically or in their valence and arousal ratings to food stimuli from NP, indicating unimpaired cognitive and emotional processing of food stimuli. This is accompanied by a cerebral neural activation pattern which is similar to NP, i.e. which is not different between groups, even when applying a very lenient initial height threshold statistically. These data are comparable to two recent studies using a food paradigm [21, 23].

In contrast to previous studies [22, 25], correlation and interaction analysis showed no significant results. However, although our study has a larger sample size than the previous ones [22, 25], it is possible that the effects are still too small to be detected with the group size of this study [70].

Comparing REC and AN, experiential data differed. The BOLD responses showed no regions of higher activation of REC as suggested by Uher et al. [24]. Applying a lenient initial height threshold, we however also found increased activation in occipital areas in AN when compared to REC, but this was located on the left side, while in the other study it was right-sided [24]. With respect to the meaning of possible occipital differences, these do not represent brain areas associated with emotional and motivational processing, but visual processing and might be unspecific [43]. Finally, we point to a recent report on neurobiological markers of 55 REC, which did not find differences between NP and REC, which can be regarded as a complementary finding to our results [71].

Not only neuronal and glial damage appear to be state phenomena [4, 71] but, according to our results, neural processing and affective rating of food stimuli also seem to be state-related. Certain neuropsychological characteristics seem to persist in some patients with AN after clinical recovery, such as difficulties in set-shifting abilities and weaker central coherence [72]. Consequently, functional cerebral aberrations are likely no candidates for endophenotypes in AN.

The study has limitations. REC were slightly older than the other groups, which is due to the course of the illness. When including age as a covariate, the results remained significant. Larger cohorts and in particular longitudinal intra-individual designs will complement our knowledge — ideally through repeated measurements. However, AN is a disease with low prevalence and high chronicity, and therefore it is difficult to recruit large enough samples — in particular in single-center studies. A further limitation of the current study represents that AN consumed less calories before the imaging protocol — which, however, is also difficult to compensate/equalize methodically. Due to the differences in calorie intake between AN and REC/NP, food processing might be due to a temporary situation of hunger during measurement and not representing a stable finding in acute AN. Although our findings suggest a restitution of brain function with regard to food stimuli, the paradigm might not be sensitive enough to detect weaker effects. Furthermore, it cannot be ruled out that there may be other areas where abnormalities persist, such as in the perception of one's own body.

Conclusions

In summary, similar to the restitution of structural cerebral abnormalities [5, 20] and serum neuronal biomarkers [71], functional brain aberrations also seem to be a state phenomenon, at least in terms of processing of food stimuli. However, this should be proven experimentally by longitudinal studies in AN and larger cohorts, which is not easily feasible methodically. Replicability is affected by several methodological issues, which we discussed in more detail elsewhere [45]. From a clinical perspective, the restitution of structural and functional cerebral alterations is an interesting issue concerning the transition of neuroscientific knowledge into clinical practice [73].

Availability of data and materials

Data are available from the senior authors on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

AN:

Anorexia nervosa

BDI:

Beck depression inventory

BOLD:

Blood oxygenation level dependent

BMI:

Body mass index

EDE:

Eating disorder examination

EDI:

Eating disorder inventory

fMRI:

Functional magnetic resonance imaging

FOV:

Field of view

MPRAGE:

Magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo

MNI:

Montreal Neurological Institute

MWT:

Multiple choice vocabulary test

NP:

Non-patients

REC:

Recovered

SCID:

Structured clinical interview for DSM

SPM:

Statistical parametric mapping

STAI:

State trait anxiety inventory

TE:

Echo time

TR:

Repetition time

References

  1. Steinhausen H-C. The outcome of anorexia nervosa in the 20th century. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:1284–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Zipfel S, Giel KE, Bulik CM, Hay P, Schmidt U. Anorexia nervosa: aetiology, assessment, and treatment. Lancet Psychiatry. 2015;2:1099–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Treasure J, Schmidt U. The cognitive-interpersonal maintenance model of anorexia nervosa revisited: a summary of the evidence for cognitive, socio-emotional and interpersonal predisposing and perpetuating factors. J Eat Disord. 2013;1:13.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Seitz J, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, Konrad K. Brain morphological changes in adolescent and adult patients with anorexia nervosa. J Neural Transm. 2016;123:949–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Nickel K, Joos A, van Elst LT, Matthis J, Holovics L, Endres D, et al. Recovery of cortical volume and thickness after remission from acute anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord. 2018;51:1056–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Simon JJ, Stopyra MA, Friederich H-C. Neural processing of disorder-related stimuli in patients with anorexia nervosa: a narrative review of brain imaging studies. J Clin Med [Internet]. 2019 [cited 2020 Apr 25];8. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6678397/

  7. Steward T, Menchón JM, Jiménez-Murcia S, Soriano-Mas C, Fernández-Aranda F. Neural network alterations across eating disorders: a narrative review of fMRI studies. Curr Neuropharmacol. 2018;16:1150–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Su T, Gong J, Tang G, Qiu S, Chen P, Chen G, et al. Structural and functional brain alterations in anorexia nervosa: a multimodal meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp. 2021;42:5154–69.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. King JA, Frank GKW, Thompson PM, Ehrlich S. Structural neuroimaging of anorexia nervosa: future directions in the quest for mechanisms underlying dynamic alterations. Biol Psychiatry. 2018;83:224–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Gottesman II, Gould TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. AJP Am Psychiatr Publ. 2003;160:636–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Erdur L, Weber C, Zimmermann-Viehoff F, Rose M, Deter H-C. Affective responses in different stages of anorexia nervosa: results from a startle-reflex paradigm. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2017;25:114–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Gárriz M, Andrés-Perpiñá S, Plana MT, Flamarique I, Romero S, Julià L, et al. Personality disorder traits, obsessive ideation and perfectionism 20 years after adolescent-onset anorexia nervosa: a recovered study. Eat Weight Disord. 2021;26:667–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Kodama N, Moriguchi Y, Takeda A, Maeda M, Ando T, Kikuchi H, et al. Neural correlates of body comparison and weight estimation in weight-recovered anorexia nervosa: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biopsychosoc Med [Internet]. 2018 [cited 2020 Apr 25];12. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6208027/

  14. Maier S, Nickel K, Perlov E, Kukies A, Zeeck A, Tebartz van Elst L, et al. Insular cell integrity markers linked to weight concern in anorexia nervosa—an MR-spectroscopy study. J Clin Med [Internet]. 2020 [cited 2020 Nov 10];9. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7288299/

  15. von Schwanenflug N, von Müller DK, King JA, Ritschel F, Bernardoni F, Mohammadi S, et al. Dynamic changes in white matter microstructure in anorexia nervosa: findings from a longitudinal study. Psychol Med. 2019;49:1555–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Bentz M, Jepsen JRM, Kjaersdam Telléus G, Moslet U, Pedersen T, Bulik CM, et al. Neurocognitive functions and social functioning in young females with recent-onset anorexia nervosa and recovered individuals. J Eat Disord. 2017;5:5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Kanakam N, Treasure J. A review of cognitive neuropsychiatry in the taxonomy of eating disorders: State, trait, or genetic? Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2013;18:83–114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Miles S, Gnatt I, Phillipou A, Nedeljkovic M. Cognitive flexibility in acute anorexia nervosa and after recovery: A systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2020;81:101905.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Nickel K, Tebartz van Elst L, Holovics L, Feige B, Glauche V, Fortenbacher T, et al. White matter abnormalities in the corpus callosum in acute and recovered anorexia nervosa patients—a diffusion tensor imaging study. Front Psychiatry [Internet]. 2019 [cited 2020 Nov 10];10. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6628864/

  20. Seitz J, Konrad K, Herpertz-Dahlmann B. Extend, pathomechanism and clinical consequences of brain volume changes in anorexia nervosa. Curr Neuropharmacol. 2018;16:1164–73.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Boehm I, Mohr H, King JA, Steding J, Geisler D, Wronski M-L, et al. Aberrant neural representation of food stimuli in women with acute anorexia nervosa predicts treatment outcome and is improved in weight restored individuals. Transl Psychiatry. 2021;11:1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Oberndorfer TA, Simmons A, McCurdy D, Strigo I, Matthews S, Yang T, et al. Greater anterior insula activation during anticipation of food images in women recovered from anorexia nervosa versus controls. Psychiatry Res [Internet]. 2013 [cited 2020 Jul 9];214. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3880160/

  23. Scaife JC, Godier LR, Reinecke A, Harmer CJ, Park RJ. Differential activation of the frontal pole to high vs low calorie foods: the neural basis of food preference in anorexia nervosa? Psychiatry Res. 2016;258:44–53.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Uher R, Brammer MJ, Murphy T, Campbell IC, Ng VW, Williams SCR, et al. Recovery and chronicity in anorexia nervosa: brain activity associated with differential outcomes. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54:934–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Holsen LM, Lawson EA, Blum J, Ko E, Makris N, Fazeli PK, et al. Food motivation circuitry hypoactivation related to hedonic and nonhedonic aspects of hunger and satiety in women with active anorexia nervosa and weight-restored women with anorexia nervosa. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2012;37:322–32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Sanders N, Smeets PAM, van Elburg AA, Danner UN, van Meer F, Hoek HW, et al. Altered food-cue processing in chronically Ill and recovered women with anorexia nervosa. Front Behav Neurosci [Internet]. 2015 [cited 2020 Apr 23];9. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4342866/

  27. Cowdrey FA, Park RJ, Harmer CJ, McCabe C. Increased neural processing of rewarding and aversive food stimuli in recovered anorexia nervosa. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70:736–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Kaye WH, Wierenga CE, Bischoff-Grethe A, Berner LA, Ely AV, Bailer UF, et al. Neural insensitivity to the effects of hunger in women remitted from anorexia nervosa. AJP. 2020;177:601–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Oberndorfer TA, Frank GKW, Simmons AN, Wagner A, McCurdy D, Fudge JL, et al. Altered insula response to sweet taste processing after recovery from anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170:1143–51.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Wagner A, Aizenstein H, Mazurkewicz L, Fudge J, Frank GK, Putnam K, et al. Altered insula response to taste stimuli in individuals recovered from restricting-type anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:513–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Frank GKW, Shott ME, Keffler C, Cornier M-A. Extremes of eating are associated with reduced neural taste discrimination. Int J Eat Disord. 2016;49:603–12.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Radeloff D, Willmann K, Otto L, Lindner M, Putnam K, Leeuwen SV, et al. High-fat taste challenge reveals altered striatal response in women recovered from bulimia nervosa: a pilot study. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2014;15:307–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. King JA, Bernardoni F, Geisler D, Ritschel F, Doose A, Pauligk S, et al. Intact value-based decision-making during intertemporal choice in women with remitted anorexia nervosa? An fMRI study. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2020;45:108–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Maier S, Schneider K, Stark C, Zeeck A, Tebartz van Elst L, Holovics L, et al. Fear network unresponsiveness in women with anorexia nervosa. PPS. 2019;88:238–40.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Maier S, Spiegelberg J, van Zutphen L, Zeeck A, van Elst LT, Hartmann A, et al. Neurobiological signature of intimacy in anorexia nervosa. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2019;27:315–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Olsavsky AK, Shott ME, DeGuzman MC, Frank GKW. Neural correlates of taste reward value across eating disorders. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2019;288:76–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Decker JH, Figner B, Steinglass JE. On weight and waiting: delay discounting in anorexia nervosa pre- and post-treatment. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;78:606–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Doose A, King JA, Bernardoni F, Geisler D, Hellerhoff I, Weinert T, et al. Strengthened default mode network activation during delay discounting in adolescents with anorexia nervosa after partial weight restoration: a longitudinal fMRI study. J Clin Med. 2020;9:900.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Castro-Fornieles J, de la Serna E, Calvo A, Blázquez A, Moya J, Lázaro L, et al. Functional MRI with a set-shifting task in adolescent anorexia nervosa: a cross-sectional and follow-up study. Neuropsychologia. 2019;131:1–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Castro-Fornieles J, Caldú X, Andrés-Perpiñá S, Lázaro L, Bargalló N, Falcón C, et al. A cross-sectional and follow-up functional MRI study with a working memory task in adolescent anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:4111–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. DeGuzman M, Shott ME, Yang TT, Riederer J, Frank GKW. Association of elevated reward prediction error response with weight gain in adolescent anorexia nervosa. AJP. 2017;174:557–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Schulte-Rüther M, Mainz V, Fink GR, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, Konrad K. Theory of mind and the brain in anorexia nervosa: relation to treatment outcome. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51:832-841.e11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Uher R, Murphy T, Brammer MJ, Dalgleish T, Phillips ML, Ng VW, et al. Medial Prefrontal cortex activity associated with symptom provocation in eating disorders. AJP. 2004;161:1238–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Joos AAB, Saum B, van Elst LT, Perlov E, Glauche V, Hartmann A, et al. Amygdala hyperreactivity in restrictive anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging. 2011;191:189–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Horster I, Nickel K, Holovics L, Schmidt S, Endres D, Tebartz van Elst L, et al. A Neglected topic in neuroscience: replicability of fMRI results with specific reference to ANOREXIA NERVOSA. Front Psychiatry [Internet]. Frontiers; 2020 [cited 2020 Sep 22];11. Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00777/full?&utm_source=Email_to_authors_&utm_medium=Email&utm_content=T1_11.5e1_author&utm_campaign=Email_publication&field=&journalName=Frontiers_in_Psychiatry&id=567828

  46. Giel KE, Conzelmann A, Renner TJ, Richter T, Benito SM, Zipfel S, et al. Attention allocation to illness-compatible information discriminates women with active versus weight-recovered anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord. 2020;53:1270–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Hilbert A, Tuschen-Caffier B, Ohms M. Eating disorder examination: Deutschsprachige Version des strukturierten Essstörungsinterviews. Diagnostica Hogrefe Verlag. 2004;50:98–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Fydrich T, Renneberg B, Schmitz B, Wittchen H-U. SKID-II Strukturiertes klinisches interview für DSM-IV: Achse II: Persönlichkeitsstörungen: Interviewheft. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 1997. p. 36.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Wittchen H-U, Zaudig M, Fydrich T. Strukturiertes Klinisches interview für DSM-IV. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Hilbert A, Tuschen-Caffier B, Karwautz A, Niederhofer H, Munsch S. Eating disorder examination-questionnaire. Diagnostica. 2007;53:144–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Hautzinger M, Keller F, Kühner C, Beck AT. Beck depressions-inventar BDI II.Revision [Internet]. Revision. Frankfurt am Main: Harcourt Test Services; 2006. Available from: https://katalog.ub.uni-freiburg.de/link?kid=1342959973

  52. Paul T, Thiel A. Eating disorder inventory-2—deutsche Fassung. Göttingen: Hogrefe [Internet]. 2005 [cited 2021 Jan 26]; Available from: http://www.redi-bw.de/db/ebsco.php/search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx%3fdirect%3dtrue%26db%3dpdx%26AN%3dPT9003604%26site%3dehost-live

  53. Laux L, Spielberger CD. Das state-trait-angstinventar STAI [Internet]. Göttingen: Beltz; 2001. Available from: https://katalog.ub.uni-freiburg.de/link?kid=1613862806

  54. Lehrl S. Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest MWT-B [Internet]. 5., unveränd. Aufl. Balingen: Spitta; 2005. Available from: https://katalog.ub.uni-freiburg.de/link?kid=1612168906

  55. Bradley MM, Lang PJ. International affective picture system. In: Zeigler-Hill V, Shackelford TK, editors. Encyclopedia of personality and individual differences [Internet]. Cham: Springer; 2017. p. 1–4. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-28099-8_42-1.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  56. Zaitsev M, Hennig J, Speck O. Point spread function mapping with parallel imaging techniques and high acceleration factors: fast, robust, and flexible method for echo-planar imaging distortion correction. Magn Reson Med. 2004;52:1156–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Friston KJ, Holmes AP, Worsley KJ, Poline J-P, Frith CD, Frackowiak RSJ. Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear approach. Hum Brain Mapp. 1994;2:189–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Rolls ET, Huang C-C, Lin C-P, Feng J, Joliot M. Automated anatomical labelling atlas 3. Neuroimage. 2020;206:116189.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Roiser JP, Linden DE, Gorno-Tempini ML, Moran RJ, Dickerson BC, Grafton ST. Minimum statistical standards for submissions to Neuroimage: Clinical. NeuroImage Clin. 2016;12:1045–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Eklund A, Knutsson H, Nichols TE. Cluster failure revisited: Impact of first level design and physiological noise on cluster false positive rates. Hum Brain Mapp. 2019;40:2017–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Woo C-W, Krishnan A, Wager TD. Cluster-extent based thresholding in fMRI analyses: pitfalls and recommendations. Neuroimage. 2014;91:412–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Frank GKW, Favaro A, Marsh R, Ehrlich S, Lawson EA. Toward valid and reliable brain imaging results in eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord. 2018;51:250–61.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Hüffmeier J, Mazei J, Schultze T. Reconceptualizing replication as a sequence of different studies: a replication typology. J Exp Soc Psycholo. 2016;66:9–10.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Bennett CM, Miller MB. How reliable are the results from functional magnetic resonance imaging? Ann NY Acad Sci. 2010;1191:133–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Gorgolewski KJ, Poldrack RA. A practical guide for improving transparency and reproducibility in neuroimaging research. PLOS Biol. 2016;14:e1002506.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Bardone-Cone AM, Harney MB, Maldonado CR, Lawson MA, Robinson DP, Smith R, et al. Defining recovery from an eating disorder: conceptualization, validation, and examination of psychosocial functioning and psychiatric comorbidity. Behav Res Ther. 2010;48:194–202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Wade TD, Lock J. Developing consensus on the definition of remission and recovery for research. Int J Eat Disord. 2020;53:1204–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Couturier J, Lock J. What is remission in adolescent anorexia nervosa? A review of various conceptualizations and quantitative analysis. Int J Eat Disord. 2006;39:175–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Khalsa SS, Portnoff LC, McCurdy-McKinnon D, Feusner JD. What happens after treatment? A systematic review of relapse, remission, and recovery in anorexia nervosa. J Eat Disord [Internet]. 2017 [cited 2020 Nov 3];5. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5470198/

  70. Elliott ML, Knodt AR, Ireland D, Morris ML, Poulton R, Ramrakha S, et al. What is the test-retest reliability of common task-functional MRI measures? New empirical evidence and a meta-analysis. Psychol Sci. 2020;31:792–806.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Doose A, Hellerhoff I, Tam FI, King JA, Seidel M, Geisler D, et al. Neural and glial damage markers in women after long-term weight-recovery from anorexia nervosa. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2022;135:105576.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Fuglset TS. Set-shifting, central coherence and decision-making in individuals recovered from anorexia nervosa: a systematic review. J Eat Disord. 2019;7:22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Bang L, Treasure J, Rø Ø, Joos A. Advancing our understanding of the neurobiology of anorexia nervosa: translation into treatment. J Eat Disord. 2017;5:38.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. German Research Foundation, JO 744/2-1 (Principal Investigator: A. Joos).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors contributed to and reviewed the study and manuscript and gave consent to its publication. All authors approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kathrin Nickel.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Freiburg (EK 520/13). Study participants gave written informed consent to participate in the study.

Consent for publication

All authors gave consent to the publication of the manuscript.

Competing interests

SG: no conflict of interest, KN: no conflict of interest, IH: no conflict of interest, DE: no conflict of interest, AZ: no conflict of interest, KD: Member of the ‘Steering Committee Neurosciences’, Janssen Pharmaceuticals, Inc., CL: no conflict of interest, LTvE: Advisory boards, lectures, or travel grants within the last three years: Roche, Eli Lilly, Janssen-Cilag, Novartis, Shire, UCB, GSK, Servier, Janssen and Cyberonics, SM: no conflict of interest, AJ: no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional

file 1: Overview of previous studies.

Additional

file 2: Results of between-group differences for the contrast food > non-food.

Additional

file 3: Results of the linear regression models for the affective ratings.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Göller, S., Nickel, K., Horster, I. et al. State or trait: the neurobiology of anorexia nervosa — contributions of a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Eat Disord 10, 77 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-022-00598-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-022-00598-7

Keywords