Skip to main content

How evaluative pairings improve body dissatisfaction in adult women: evidence from a randomized-controlled online study

Abstract

Background

Many young women are dissatisfied with their bodies. This study investigated the effect on current body dissatisfaction levels of a newly developed evaluative conditioning procedure that paired self-similar and self-dissimilar images of bodies with positive and neutral affective images, respectively. We hypothesized that learning the contingency that self-similar bodies predict positive affectivity is one process that could aid in explaining how these procedures function.

Methods

Adult women without disordered eating pathology participated in an online experiment with random assignment to an intervention or a control condition. All participants initially rated body images in self-similarity and were subsequently asked to categorize positive and neutral images by valence as quickly and accurately as possible. In the intervention condition, self-similar bodies systematically preceded positive images, and self-dissimilar images preceded neutral images, creating a similar body → positive contingency. Pairings in the control condition were unsystematic such that no contingency was present. We measured categorization latencies and accuracies to infer contingency learning as well as current body dissatisfaction immediately before and after exposure to the pairings. All participants further completed measures of trait body image concerns and disordered eating psychopathology at baseline, which we examined as moderators of an expected relation between condition assignment, contingency learning, and body dissatisfaction improvements.

Results

We analyzed data from N = 173 women fulfilling the inclusion criteria. Moderated mediation analyses showed that assignment to the intervention (vs. control) condition predicted increased similar body → positive contingency learning, which in turn predicted improved body dissatisfaction post-intervention, but only among women with higher pre-existing trait body image concerns or disordered eating levels.

Conclusions

The findings point toward the relevancy of further exploring the utility of pairing procedures. Similar body → positive contingency learning predicted improved body dissatisfaction in individuals with normatively high body image concerns, which suggests pairing procedures could help inform future research on reducing body dissatisfaction.

Plain English summary

Many people are dissatisfied with how their bodies look or how much they weigh. Body dissatisfaction can increase the risk of developing an eating disorder. This study tested a method for reducing body dissatisfaction among women. The method included pairing pictures of bodies judged as similar to one’s own body with positive pictures. For one half of the study participants, we arranged the pairings in a way that one could systematically learn that similar body pictures and positive pictures go together. Compared to the other half of study participants who were shown pairings by chance, we found that study participants indeed learned that similar body pictures and positive pictures go together. Moreover, this learning made participants who were generally dissatisfied with their bodies or who reported disordered eating symptoms more satisfied with their bodies immediately after the procedure. These findings suggest that the method could be further developed, investigated, and used in treating or preventing eating disorders.

Introduction

Many young women are dissatisfied with their bodies, including body shape, size, muscularity, and weight [1, 2]. Body dissatisfaction can originate from internalizing unrealistic body ideals [3], comparisons on social media [4], and has been associated with childhood maltreatment [5]. Substantial levels of body dissatisfaction affect between 11 and 72% of women [6, 7], and pose an increased risk for disordered eating and weight-control behaviors [8, 9], lower rates of health-promoting behaviors (e.g., cancer screenings [10], and even increased mortality [11,12,13]. Given these potentially severe consequences, investigating novel techniques to alleviate body dissatisfaction is crucial.

Existing standalone interventions for improving body dissatisfaction frequently target antecedent and maintaining factors of negative body images. These can be grouped into cognitive-behavioral techniques (e.g., cognitive restructuring, exposure exercises), interventions providing media literacy (e.g., discussing beauty ideals), enhancing general self-esteem (e.g., social comparison exercises), and psychoeducation [14]. Although overall effective [14], the effect sizes corrected for publication bias of body dissatisfaction improvements found in a comprehensive meta-analysis were small (i.e., Cohen’s d = 0.15, 95% CI [0.02; 0.28]), indicating a need to develop and explore further interventions.

A promising candidate for improving body dissatisfaction directly is evaluative conditioning or pairing procedures [15]. Evaluative conditioning has been defined as the change in liking (i.e., the subjective appraisal as positive or negative) of a target conditioned stimulus (CS) due to its pairing with a liked or disliked source, i.e., the unconditioned stimulus (US; [16]. In a prototypical study, target stimuli such as artificial brand names are repeatedly presented for a few seconds with either positive or negative source stimuli (e.g., pictures, words). Statistically, the repeated pairings create a contingency between two events (the occurrence of a target stimulus predicts the occurrence of a positive or negative source stimulus). The pairings typically lead to changes in the evaluation of the targets, usually assessed immediately afterwards, such that targets paired with liked sources are evaluated more favorably compared to both unpaired targets and targets paired with disliked sources [17].

Given that pairing procedures have been frequently used to change liking across different domains [18, 19], Martijn et al. [20] adapted a pairing procedure to improve current (state-like) body dissatisfaction in young women. In a first session, participants’ full body photos were taken in standardized clothing and body dissatisfaction was assessed. After eight days, the photos were repeatedly paired in one condition as target stimuli (CS) with smiling faces as evaluative sources (US; other body images were paired with neutral and frowning faces as control target and source stimuli, respectively). The control condition consisted of unsystematic pairings in which neither the own nor other body images predicted the presentation of smiling faces. Compared to that, the own body → positive contingency in the intervention condition improved current body dissatisfaction levels, assessed directly after the conditioning procedure, in the small-to-medium range. However, medium-to-large increases were observed for women with high levels of pre-existing (trait-like) body image concerns. Martijn et al. [20] suggested that social recognition and acceptance could be more important for women with high, trait-like body image concerns than those with low body image concerns. Pairing one’s body with positive stimuli could effectively simulate social recognition, explaining the procedure’s pronounced effect among women with higher body image concerns.

Aspen et al. [21] and Kollei et al. [22] provide further evidence that pairing own body images with positive stimuli improves body dissatisfaction. Both studies employed pairing sessions repeatedly (over the course of four or two weeks, respectively), and demonstrated effects with prolonged follow-ups (four and twelve, or four weeks, respectively). However, several more recent findings challenge these effects. For example, pairing own body photos with smiling faces in conceptual (six pairing sessions over three weeks) and direct replication studies of the Martijn et al. procedure did not improve current body dissatisfaction among women with [23] and without eating disorders [24], irrespective of a delayed or immediate body dissatisfaction assessment. Similar studies among female university students in which own face and body pictures were paired with images of other women’s smiling faces in repeated sessions over one week [25] or emojis in a single session [26] could also not find that current body dissatisfaction improved in neither a delayed nor an immediate assessment, irrespective of the pre-existing levels of body image concerns.

While the overall heterogeneous findings could question the utility of pairing procedures for improving body dissatisfaction, it is important to note that previous studies rarely examined the mediating processes (for an overview of processes, see [17]. For example, previous studies rarely examined the role of encoding the manipulated body → positive contingency (i.e., contingency learning), which could be one relevant mechanism to improving body dissatisfaction [27] (notably, both Martijn et al. [20], and Glashouwer et al. [24], had participants estimate the manipulated contingency, but did not conduct mediation analyses). Indeed, selectively attending to both target and source stimuli is a pre-requisite for contingency learning [28], and several previous experimental studies show that the success of encoding the target-source contingency can predict the effectiveness of pairing procedures (e.g., [29,30,31]. We would therefore expect that evaluative pairing effects for improving body satisfaction could depend on learning (or failing to learn) the intended target → source contingency.

For example, participants in Kosinski et al. [25], who used an “app” to pair face and body pictures with images of other women’s smiling faces, may have failed to learn a body → positive contingency, given they were tasked with selectively attending to the identity (rather than positive valence) of a paired smiling face that was presented among other smiling faces [32]. Similarly, Glashouwer et al. [24] and Masselman et al. [26] had participants wear colored clothing that distinguished their body photos from control target stimuli. While aiding in standardizing the procedures and recognizing one’s body, this color-coding may have had the unintended side-effect of distracting attention away from relevant body features such as size or shape. Participants thus may have encoded a color → positive contingency instead. Although speculative and a posteriori, this line of reasoning illustrates that ensuring and assessing the encoding of a body → positive contingency could be crucial for a pairing procedure’s success in improving body dissatisfaction.

The present experiment investigated the effect of a newly designed pairing procedure to improve body dissatisfaction in women. Because previous studies show considerable variations in terms of “dosing” (the number of pairings and sessions) and follow-up delay (from immediate to twelve weeks after), we focused on a likely common denominator, i.e., the ability of a single pairing session to improve body dissatisfaction immediately. We thus operationalized body dissatisfaction in terms of a changeable state. Based on the idea that pairing effects could be driven by successfully encoding a body → positive contingency, our paradigm adopted an active evaluative pairing procedure with simultaneous contingency learning assessment [30]. The procedure included four types of target-source (i.e., CS-US) pairs: body images judged as similar or dissimilar were paired with positive and neutral pictures. However, depending on the contingency condition, the number of pairings differed. In the intervention condition, similar-positive and dissimilar-neutral pairs were in the majority, such that self-similar targets predicted the occurrence of positive images (i.e., a similar body → positive contingency was present). In the control condition, all types of pairs occurred equally often such that there was no similar body → positive contingency. Because we asked participants to categorize each positive or neutral source upon its occurrence, the successful encoding of the similar body → positive contingency could be inferred from systematic differences in response times and accuracy between predicted and unpredicted positive stimulus occurrences. Although different from previous implementations, this procedure had the advantage of using standardized stimuli throughout, including the body images, to avoid any unwanted visual distinctions or distractions that could interfere with encoding the intended similar body → positive contingency.

We hypothesized that a) the similar body → positive contingency would predict shorter response times and higher accuracy regarding positive image categorization in the intervention compared to the control condition and that b) these differences, as an index of similar body → positive contingency learning, would predict improvements in current body dissatisfaction due to the pairing procedure (i.e., we hypothesized that the effect of condition assignment on body dissatisfaction improvement is mediated by differences in contingency learning induced by the differences between the conditions). Moreover, because previous research showed that pre-existing, trait-like body image concerns could increase the impact of pairing procedures on state-like body dissatisfaction improvements [20,21,22], we further explored whether c) the effect of similar body → positive contingency learning on current body dissatisfaction improvements would increase with increasing levels of pre-existing body image concerns.

Materials and methods

Participants and design

We recruited 182 adult women (Mage = 31.4, age range: 18–64 years) for an online study from university and social network forums and among colleagues, friends, and acquaintances, to be randomly assigned to the conditions of a 2 (group: intervention vs. control) between-participants design. To homogenize and align the sample with previous studies [20, 24], we excluded women who disclosed a history of an eating disorder or who were currently receiving treatment for disordered eating. We initially envisioned including only healthy women with at least mild body image concerns, but dropped this criterion due to validity concerns (i.e., we intended to use a response toward a single question, which had not been validated) and because limiting the variability in the sample reduces the power for detecting moderations.

We targeted a minimum sample size of N = 128 to achieve a power of 1−β = 0.80 to detect a medium-sized difference (f = 0.25) between intervention vs. control condition for body dissatisfaction changes at α = 0.05. All data were collected from April 2022 to January 2023. Data and materials can be obtained from the corresponding author upon request. We report all measures, manipulations, exclusions and deviations from pre-registration (see Declarations).

Measures and procedure

We implemented the study in jsPsych [33]. Upon accessing the study’s website, participants were randomly assigned to the intervention or control condition and read the study information and consent forms. Study participation required a physical keyboard. After providing informed consent, participants self-reported their age, weight, height, regular medications via open-ended questions and gender, language ability, years of education, dominant hand, pregnancy status, history of eating disorders, current eating disorder treatment, and previous study participation via multiple-choice questions. We then assessed, in random order, disordered eating symptoms via the German short version of the Eating Disorder Examination-Questionnaire (EDE-Q8, [34] as a control measure [20,21,22,23, 25, 26] and, more importantly, pre-existing trait-like body image concerns as a potential moderator of pairing procedure effects using the German Body Shape Questionnaire (Fragebogen zum Figurbewusstsein, BSQ; [35]. The BSQ´s 34 items assessed thoughts and concerns about one’s body shape during the past 28 days on a scale from 1 (never) to 6 (always), with the sum score serving as a validated measure of (trait-like) body image concerns.

Target body selection

After completing the initial assessments, participants commenced to the main procedure. First, participants rated 44 standardized, black-and-white photographs of eleven women’s bodies (without heads and in neutral underwear), taken from four perspectives (standing-front, standing-back, standing-side, and sitting-side; for examples, see [36]. The models’ Body-Mass-Index (BMI) ranged from 13.8 to 61.3 kg/m2, and participants rated How similar does this body looks to your body? on a visual analog scale (VAS) ranging from -100 (not similar at all) to 100 (highly similar). We used each participant’s ratings to select the bodies with the highest and the median average similarity to serve as similar and dissimilar target stimuli, respectively. We chose the median rather than the least similar body for comparison, as we expected severely underweight or overweight bodies always to receive the lowest similarity ratings. Selecting the least similar body may thus have unintentionally confounded similarity with a specific weight dimension.

Baseline state body dissatisfaction

Next, participants completed the German Body Image State Scales (BISS, [37, 38], which was also used in recent studies [23, 24, 26] and thus served as this study’s change-sensitive measure of state-like body dissatisfaction. The mean across its six items measured the current (“right now, in this very moment”) evaluation and affect about the physical appearance and attractiveness using different 9-point scales (from − 4, very dissatisfied/very unattractive/very much worse than usual to 4, very satisfied/very attractive/very much better than usual). We coded the scale such that higher scores indicate a more positive body evaluation.

Pairing task and contingency learning assessment

After completing the BISS, we explained that the next task would show photos of bodies as well as positive and neutral images. Each task trial (see Fig. 1) started with the display of a fixation cross presented for 500, 1000, 1500, or 2000 ms (selected at random). Next, one of four similar or dissimilar body images was displayed for 1000 ms (i.e., the two selected bodies across the four perspectives), followed by the presentation of a positive or neutral target image (e.g., of cute animals, happy people, landscapes vs. daily objects). We selected a total of 48 positive and 48 neutral target images from various validated databases [39,40,41]. Upon presentation of the positive or neutral image, participants were asked to categorize it as either positive or neutral using their keyboard’s m and c keys, respectively. This ensured focusing attention on the valence of the source stimuli, which has been known to strengthen pairing procedure effects [32]. The categorization further allowed us to measure contingency learning behaviorally [30]. If categorized incorrectly, or if no response was given within 3000 ms, an error message was displayed for 500 ms before advancing to the next trial (a blank slide was displayed for correct responses). In total, the task comprised 96 trials, presented in random order, such that each positive or neutral image was displayed only once. The task took approx. 8 min to complete.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Trial sequence illustration of the pairing procedure. Each trial started with the display of a fixation, followed by a similar or dissimilar body image, then followed by the presentation of a positive or neutral target image. Participants were asked to categorize the target images upon presentation using their keyboard c and m keys. Trials were presented in random order

Critically, in the intervention condition, pictures of the self-similar body were followed mainly by positive images (40 vs. 8). In contrast, pictures of the self-dissimilar body were followed less often by positive than neutral images (8 vs. 40). Thus, the similar body → positive contingency was ϕ = 0.67 in the intervention condition. In the control condition, there was no contingency between self-similarity and the valence of the source pictures (24 trials per each combination; similar body → positive contingency ϕ = 0). However, the overall number of similar and dissimilar body images did not differ between conditions, such that the total number of stimulus exposures and response base rates remained identical.

State body dissatisfaction post-intervention and conclusion

After completing the pairing procedure, participants next completed the BISS for a second time to detect induced changes in body dissatisfaction. Finally, we asked participants to rate How attractive do you find each body? on a VAS ranging from − 100 (not at all attractive) to 100 (very attractive) for each picture presented for target body selection. We collected these ratings to explore any potential effects of the procedure on a perceived shift in body attractiveness [36]. After these ratings, participants were thanked and dismissed.

Data aggregation and analysis

We extracted relative frequencies and median latencies of correct responses (i.e., response accuracy and response time in ms) for each participant from the data collected during contingency learning. These were submitted to two separate 2 (group: intervention vs. control) × 2 (target body: similar vs. dissimilar) × 2 (source valence: positive vs. neutral) analyses of variance (ANOVAs), with target body and source valence as repeated-measures factors, to assess the predicted effect of contingency learning.

The questionnaires showed high levels of internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.96, 0.91, and 0.91/0.92 for the BSQ, EDE-Q8, and BISS baseline/post-intervention, respectively) and were thus aggregated according to their convention. We compared baseline scores (as well as participant characteristics) between conditions, and before and after the intervention with independent samples t-tests. Following recommendations [42], the effect on BISS scores post-intervention was examined in a 2 (group: intervention vs. control) analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), with baseline scores as covariate, as well as in a mediation analysis with contingency learning as a mediator and trait-like body image concerns as a moderator of the effect of contingency learning on current body dissatisfaction improvement (again, baseline BISS scores were included as covariate). We initially intended to include BSQ scores as a covariate in the ANCOVA, but omitted reporting this analysis as it yielded comparable effects, further moderations were described as exploratory analyses in the study registration.

The significance level for all analyses was set at p ≤ 0.05. Post hoc pairwise comparisons report Bonferroni-adjusted p-values for multiple comparisons. Effect sizes are reported as ηp2. Variable values are reported as means and standard deviations (SDs). The data were aggregated and analyzed with IBM SPSS 28 [43]. We used PROCESS v4.2 model 14 [44] with boot-strapped (10,000 samples) bias-corrected 95% confidence interval (CI) for moderated mediation analysis. The effects of the moderator were evaluated at its mean (M) and at M ± 1 SD.

Results

Participant characteristics

We analyzed the data from 173 women (Mage = 30.9, age range: 18–64 years) assigned to the intervention (n = 84) and control condition (n = 89), respectively, after excluding participants that did not fulfill the inclusion criteria. The included sample sociodemographic and baseline characteristics are summarized in Table 1. The conditions did not differ in age, BMI, BSQ scores, EDE-Q8 scores, or baseline BISS scores, all ps > 0.35.

Table 1 Participant sociodemographic and baseline descriptive statistics (mean and SD or n and percentage)

Contingency learning

Mean response accuracies are depicted in Fig. 2. The mixed-measures ANOVA revealed a group × target body interaction, F(1, 171) = 5.79, p = 0.02, ηp2 = 0.03, and a target body × source valence interaction, F(1, 171) = 14.82, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.08, which were qualified by the group × target body × source valence interaction, F(1, 171) = 7.23, p = 0.008, ηp2 = 0.04; all other Fs < 3.1, all ps > 0.08. As predicted, pairwise comparisons showed an increased frequency of correct responses for positive images in the intervention condition following similar bodies vs. dissimilar bodies, p < 0.001. Likewise, we found a decreased frequency of correct responses for neutral images following similar vs. dissimilar bodies, p = 0.014. In other words, response accuracies in the intervention condition reflected the manipulated similar body → positive contingency, with predicted source stimulus occurrences categorized more accurately than non-predicted occurrences. In comparison, response accuracy for positive and neutral images did not differ in the control condition as a function of the target body, ps > 0.33, consistent with the absence of a similar body → positive contingency in that condition.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Relative frequencies of correct responses during similar body → positive contingency learning. Similar bodies predicted the occurrence of positive images in the intervention condition; there was no contingency between type of body and affective image in the control condition

A second mixed-measures ANOVA of response latencies revealed that positive images were correctly categorized faster than neutral images (M = 847 ms, SD = 216 vs. M = 884 ms, SD = 210), F(1, 170) = 22.57, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.12. Other effects were not significant, all other Fs < 2.5, ps > 0.11.

Changes in state body dissatisfaction

BISS scores increased from baseline to after the intervention (Mt1 = 0.89, SD = 1.54 vs. Mt2 = 0.99, SD = 1.51), t(172) = 2.16, p = 0.03, d = 0.17, but the ANCOVA did not find an advantage of the intervention over the control condition post-intervention (M = 1.01, SD = 0.61 vs. M = 0.98, SD = 0.61, respectively), F(1, 170) = 0.10, p = 0.76. However, we hypothesized that pairing effects on state body dissatisfaction should be driven by successfully encoding a similar body → positive contingency (and we also intended to explore that pre-existing body image concerns further moderate this effect). We therefore investigated the hypothesized mediation pattern next. For this analysis, we calculated the individual differences between correct categorizations of positive images following similar vs. dissimilar target bodies as the index of similar body → positive contingency learning.

The moderated mediation model with group as predictor X (coded 1 0, for intervention and control conditions, respectively), similar body → positive contingency learning as mediator M, BISS scores post-intervention as criterion Y (with BISS baseline as covariate), and BSQ scores as moderator W of the effect of contingency learning, is displayed in Fig. 3. Consistent with the previous analysis, group predicted similar body → positive contingency learning, bXM = 0.07, p < 0.001. Contingency learning, in turn, interacted with BSQ scores to predict BISS change, bM*WY = 0.03, p = 0.04, R2Change = 0.004. Simple slopes showed that, at lower and mean BSQ levels, the effects of contingency learning on BISS change were not significant, bMY = − 0.34, p = 0.49 and bMY = 0.42, p = 0.22, respectively. However, at higher BSQ levels, an increase in contingency learning predicted improvements in BISS scores, bMY = 1.17, p = 0.03 (for Johnson-Neyman significance regions, see Fig. 4). The boot-strapped model confirmed the hypothesized pattern: The index of moderated mediation (IMM) was significant, IMM = 0.002, 95% CI [0.001; 0.004], with an indirect effect (IE) observed for the high BSQ level, IE = 0.09, 95% CI [0.01; 0.20], but not at mean and lower BSQ levels, IEs = 0.03, 95% CI [− 0.01; 0.08] and − 0.03, 95% CI [− 0.08; 0.02], respectively. There was no direct effect (DE) of group on BISS changes when controlling for the influence of the mediator and moderator, DE = − 0.01, 95% [− 0.20; 0.18].

Fig. 3
figure 3

The moderated mediation model with group as predictor X (coded 1 0, for intervention and control conditions, respectively), similar body → positive contingency learning as mediator M, BISS post-intervention scores as criterion Y (with baseline scores as covariate), and BSQ body image scores as moderator W of the effect of contingency learning on BISS changes. The boot-strapped model confirmed that a positive effect of the intervention on contingency learning improved body dissatisfaction, but only among individuals with higher body image concerns

Fig. 4
figure 4

Johnson–Neyman Significance Regions for the Conditional Effects of Contingency Learning on State Body Dissatisfaction at Different Values of the Body Shape Questionnaire (BSQ). The effect is significant at BSQ values above the dotted, vertical line. LL = lower level, UL = upper level, CI = confidence interval

Further exploratory analyses

Eating disorder psychopathology, as assessed by the EDE-Q8, correlated significantly with BSQ sum scores, r(171) = 0.80, p < 0.001, and BISS post-intervention, r(171) = -0.68, p < 0.001. Because several previous studies used EDE-Q scores for moderator analyses [20,21,22], we similarly explored moderated mediation patterns based on the EDE-Q8. When using the mean EDE-Q8 instead of the BSQ scores as moderator, an inferentially identical moderated mediation pattern emerged (see Table 2 and Fig. 5), suggesting an increased utility of pairing procedures with increasing levels of disordered eating symptoms. Other variables, such as participant age or BMI, showed no direct or indirect associations with the pairing procedure’s effects on changes in BISS. BSQ and EDE-Q8 scores showed no direct association with contingency learning across and within conditions, all ps > 0.10.

Table 2 Coefficient estimates with boot-strapped (10,000 samples) bias-corrected 95% confidence interval (CI) for the moderated mediation model based on disordered eating psychopathology
Fig. 5
figure 5

Johnson–Neyman Significance Regions for the Conditional Effects of Contingency Learning on State Body Dissatisfaction at Different Values of the Eating Disorder Examination-Questionnaire short version (EDE-Q8). The effect is significant at EDE-Q8 values above the dotted, vertical line. LL = lower level, UL = upper level, CI = confidence interval

Finally, we investigated the potential effects on participants’ attractiveness ratings. A 2 (group) × 2 (target body) mixed-measures ANOVA showed that bodies selected as similar were rated as more attractive than bodies at median similarity (M = 129, SD = 44.20 vs. M = 80.68, SD = 55.34), F(1, 171) = 66.08, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.28. Attractiveness ratings were not directly affected by the group condition, all Fs < 1, all ps > 0.42. However, additional exploratory moderated-mediation analyses showed that the difference in attractiveness between self-similar and self-dissimilar bodies increased as a function of group with increasing contingency learning, for individuals with high BSQ or EDE-Q8 scores (see Additional file 1). In other words, we observed a moderated-mediation pattern for the direct evaluation of body images parallel to changes in state body dissatisfaction. This suggests that a more positive evaluation of self-similar bodies could indeed be responsible for the observed improvements in body dissatisfaction.

Discussion

The present study investigated the effect of a newly designed pairing procedure to improve current body dissatisfaction in women. Based on the idea that pairing effects could be partially driven by successfully encoding an own body → positive contingency, we predicted that learning a similar body → positive contingency would reduce current body dissatisfaction. Although we did not find that the similar body → positive contingency of pairings directly reduced body dissatisfaction levels, we found that participants reliably encoded the similar body → positive contingency in the intervention condition, and that body image improved for all participants (likely reflecting a mere body exposure effect; [45, 46]. Moreover, and as predicted, mediation analyses showed that learning the similar body → positive contingency, in turn, predicted current body dissatisfaction improvements for women, but only for those with high trait-like body image concerns or high disordered eating psychopathology.

The evidence for mediation reiterates the importance of assessing the mechanisms underlying the effects of pairing procedures. We proposed that learning (or failing to learn) the intended target → source contingency drives evaluative pairing effects, and speculated that similar previous studies (also different in several ways) may have failed to observe the effect of pairing participants' body images with other positive stimuli due to participants failing to learn the manipulated contingency [15]. Of course, we cannot verify these speculations a posteriori, and there are multiple mechanisms by which pairing procedures may exert a positive effect [19]. Still, the present findings support focusing efforts on improving or at least measuring relevant contingency learning in future applications.

Moreover, our findings are consistent with previous observations that the positive effects of pairing procedures on body dissatisfaction states are pronounced for individuals with pre-existing body image concern traits [20]. The moderated mediation patterns found here showed that only women with BSQ body image scores larger than 92, or EDE-Q8 scores larger 2.1, benefitted from successfully encoding the similar body → positive contingency (coincidentally, BSQ scores above 90 denote significant body image concerns according to normative data [47]. At the same time, many participants with low trait concerns (e.g., BSQ < 90) were still less than fully satisfied with their bodies prior to the intervention (M = 1.4 out of − 4 to + 4 at BISS baseline), implying that the moderation is not merely an artefact of regression to the mean. Thus, successful body → positive contingency learning may itself not be sufficient to incur improvements in state-like body dissatisfaction, and its potential for positive effects likely depends on further boundary conditions. It has been speculated that increased sensitivity to social feedback heightens body image concerns and, in turn, explains the increased efficacy of pairing procedures—as a way of mimicking social feedback—in improving body dissatisfaction [20]. Likewise, pre-existing body image concerns themselves could heighten attention towards comments, opinions, and reactions of others, that is, direct attention towards external information, and thus directly explain the increased effects of pairing procedures. The specific mechanism explaining the observed moderation thus still needs to be explored.

Limitations and avenues for future research

Despite the data supporting an overall beneficial effect of pairing procedures, it is important to note several limitations. First, while we can assume that contingency-consistent differences in categorization performance reflect contingency learning, the absence of these effects does not necessitate the absence of learning. Besides the requirement of immediately responding to the stimuli, we did not include further attention checks and thus cannot rule out that aspects other than lower contingency learning contributed to the observed effects. Second, we recruited participants largely among German university students, who are likely White, female, more affluent, and more educated compared to the general population [48]. Although students are not exempt from experiencing body dissatisfaction, it is important to note that sociodemographic features such as gender and ethnicity can affect body image concerns and related health issues [49, 50]. As we did not explicitly assess participants’ ethnicities, we cannot conclude that the here observed effects will generalize towards more diverse samples without gathering further data. Relatedly, we must note that presenting bodies as “headless”, which was an unintended side-effect of using already existing stimulus materials, could have contributed to an “objectified” view of one’s own body, potentially further limiting the generalizability of our findings. Moreover, our study has thus far been limited to investigating short-term effects on body dissatisfaction improvements. Although pairing effects are relatively resistant toward extinction [51], thus far only a few studies demonstrated more long-term effects of pairing procedures for improving body image (up to 12 weeks after intervention [21, 22]. Finally, we only investigated the effect of a single session,the effect of repeated training sessions (i.e., the dosing level) needs to be further explored.

Conclusions

Nonetheless, our findings point toward the relevancy of further exploring the utility of pairing procedures. Similar body → positive contingency learning improved current body dissatisfaction in individuals with normatively high body image concerns [47], which suggests these procedures could specifically benefit their intended audience. Moreover, the improvements were observed in addition to an overall effect of mere body exposure, which could suggest pairing procedures may ultimately augment other existing intervention strategies. Thus, further exploration of this line of research is warranted.

Availability of data and materials

Data and materials can be obtained from the corresponding author upon request.

Abbreviations

BISS:

Body image states scale

BMI:

Body mass index

BSQ:

Body shape questionnaire

CS:

Conditioned stimulus

DE:

Direct effect

EDE-Q8:

Eating disorder examination-questionnaire short version

IE:

Indirect effect

IMM:

Index of moderated mediation

US:

Unconditioned stimulus

References

  1. Neumark-Sztainer D, Wall MM, Chen C, Larson NI, Christoph MJ, Sherwood NE. Eating, activity, and weight-related problems from adolescence to adulthood. Am J Prev Med. 2018;55(2):133–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2018.04.032.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Rodgers RF, Laveway K, Campos P, de Carvalho PHB. Body image as a global mental health concern. Cambridge: Cambridge Prisms: Global Mental Health; 2023. https://doi.org/10.1017/gmh.2023.2.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  3. Stice E, Whitenton K. Risk factors for body dissatisfaction in adolescent girls: a longitudinal investigation. Dev Psychol. 2002;38(5):669–78. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.5.669.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Roberts SR, Maheux AJ, Hunt RA, Ladd BA, Choukas-Bradley S. Incorporating social media and muscular ideal internalization into the tripartite influence model of body image: towards a modern understanding of adolescent girls’ body dissatisfaction. Body Image. 2022;41:239–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2022.03.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Bödicker C, Reinckens J, Höfler M, Hoyer J. Is childhood maltreatment associated with body image disturbances in adulthood? A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Child Adolesc Trauma. 2022;15(3):523–38. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40653-021-00379-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Fiske L, Fallon EA, Blissmer B, Redding CA. Prevalence of body dissatisfaction among United States adults: review and recommendations for future research. Eat Behav. 2014;15(3):357–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014.04.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Von Lengerke T, Mielck A. Body weight dissatisfaction by socioeconomic status among obese, preobese and normal weight women and men: results of the cross-sectional KORA Augsburg S4 population survey. BMC Public Health. 2012;12(1):1. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-12-342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Eck KM, Quick V, Byrd-Bredbenner C. Body dissatisfaction, eating styles, weight-related behaviors, and health among young women in the United States. Nutrients. 2022. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14183876.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Jiménez-Limas K, Miranda-Barrera VA, Muñoz-Díaz KF, Novales-Huidobro SR, Chico-Barba G. Body dissatisfaction, distorted body image and disordered eating behaviors in university students: an analysis from 2017–2022. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191811482.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Ridolfi DR, Crowther JH. The link between women’s body image disturbances and body-focused cancer screening behaviors: a critical review of the literature and a new integrated model for women. Body Image. 2013;10(2):149–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2012.11.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Brenner H, Stock C, Hoffmeister M. Effect of screening sigmoidoscopy and screening colonoscopy on colorectal cancer incidence and mortality: systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials and observational studies. BMJ. 2014;348(apr09 1):g2467–g2467. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.g2467.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Peirson L, Fitzpatrick-Lewis D, Ciliska D, Warren R. Screening for cervical cancer: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Syst Rev. 2013. https://doi.org/10.1186/2046-4053-2-35.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Tabár L, Vitak B, Tonychen HH, Yen MF, Duffy SW, Smith RA. Beyond randomized controlled trials: organized mammographic screening substantially reduces breast carcinoma mortality. Cancer. 2001;91(9):1724–31. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-0142(20010501)91:9%3c1724::AID-CNCR1190%3e3.0.CO;2-V.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Alleva JM, Sheeran P, Webb TL, Martijn C, Miles E. A meta-analytic review of stand-alone interventions to improve body image. PLoS ONE. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0139177.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Halbeisen G, Paslakis G. All i need is two: the clinical potential of adding evaluative pairing procedures to cognitive behavioral therapy for changing self-, body-and food-related evaluations. J Clin Med. 2021;10(20):4703. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10204703.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. De Houwer J. A conceptual and theoretical analysis of evaluative conditioning. Span J Psychol Copyr. 2007;10(2):230–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Hofmann W, De Houwer J, Perugini M, Baeyens F, Crombez G. Evaluative conditioning in humans: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2010;136(3):390–421. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018916.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. De Houwer J, Thomas S, Baeyens F. Associative learning of likes and dislikes: a review of 25 years of research on human evaluative conditioning. Psychol Bull. 2001. https://doi.org/10.1037//D033-29O9.127.6.853.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Walther E, Blask K, Halbeisen G, Frings C. An action control perspective of evaluative conditioning. Eur Rev Soc Psychol. 2019;30(1):271–310. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2019.1699743.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Martijn C, Vanderlinden M, Roefs A, Huijding J, Jansen A. Increasing body satisfaction of body concerned women through evaluative conditioning using social stimuli. Health Psychol. 2010;29(5):514–20. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020770.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Aspen V, Martijn C, Alleva JM, Nagel J, Perret C, Purvis C, Saekow J, Lock J, Taylor CB. Decreasing body dissatisfaction using a brief conditioning intervention. Behav Res Ther. 2015;69:93–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2015.04.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Kollei I, Lukas CA, Loeber S, Berking M. An app-based blended intervention to reduce body dissatisfaction: a randomized controlled pilot study. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2017;85(11):1104–8. https://doi.org/10.1037/ccp0000246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Glashouwer KA, Neimeijer RAM, De Koning ML, Vestjens M, Martijn C. Evaluative conditioning as a body image intervention for adolescents with eating disorders. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2018;86(12):1046–55. https://doi.org/10.1037/ccp0000311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Glashouwer KA, Masselman I, de Jong PJ. Reducing body dissatisfaction by means of an evaluative conditioning procedure in undergraduate women: a replication study. Behav Res Ther. 2019;121(May): 103435. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2019.103435.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Kosinski T. A brief mobile evaluative conditioning app to reduce body dissatisfaction? A pilot study in university women. Front Psychol. 2019;10(November):1–8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Masselman I, de Jong PJ, Glashouwer KA. What’s not to like? Enhancing women’s body satisfaction by means of an evaluative conditioning procedure with positive social feedback. J Exp Psychopathol. 2022. https://doi.org/10.1177/20438087221085767.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Crocker J, Knight KM. Contingencies of self-worth. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2005;14(4):200–3. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.0963-7214.2005.00364.X/ASSET/IMAGES/LARGE/10.1111_J.0963-7214.2005.00364.X-FIG1.JPEG.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Blask K, Walther E, Frings C. When congruence breeds preference: the influence of selective attention processes on evaluative conditioning. Cogn Emot. 2017;31(6):1127–39. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.1197100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Aust F, Haaf JM, Stahl C. A memory-based judgment account of expectancy-liking dissociations in evaluative conditioning. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2019;45(3):417–39. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000600.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Halbeisen G, Walther E. Evaluative conditioning is sensitive to the encoding of CS-US contingencies. Soc Cogn. 2016;34(5):462–79. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2016.34.5.462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Kattner F. Reconsidering the (in)sensitivity of evaluative conditioning to reinforcement density and CS-US contingency. Learn Motiv. 2014;45(1):15–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2013.09.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Gast A, Rothermund K. What you see is what will change: evaluative conditioning effects depend on a focus on valence. Cogn Emot. 2011;25(1):89–110. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931003696380.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. de Leeuw JR, Gilbert RA, Luchterhandt B. jsPsych: enabling an open-source collaborative ecosystem of behavioral experiments. J Open Source Softw. 2023;8(85):5351. https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.05351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Kliem S, Mößle T, Zenger M, Strauß B, Brähler E, Hilbert A. The eating disorder examination-questionnaire 8: a brief measure of eating disorder psychopathology (EDE-Q8). Int J Eat Disord. 2016;49(6):613–6. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22487.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Pook M, Tuschen-Caffier B, Stich N. Evaluation des Fragebogens zum Figurbewusstsein (FFB, deutsche version des Body Shape Questionnaire). Verhaltenstherapie. 2002;12(2):116–24. https://doi.org/10.1159/000064375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Horndasch S, Heinrich H, Kratz O, Mai S, Graap H, Moll GH. Perception and evaluation of women’s bodies in adolescents and adults with anorexia nervosa. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2015;265(8):677–87. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-015-0603-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Bauer A, Schneider S, Waldorf M, Braks K, Huber TJ, Adolph D, Vocks S. Selective visual attention towards oneself and associated state body satisfaction: an eye-tracking study in adolescents with different types of eating disorders. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2017;45(8):1647–61. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-017-0263-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Cash TF, Fleming EC, Alindogan J, Steadman L, Whitehead A. Beyond body image as a trait: the development and validation of the body image states scale. Eat Disord. 2002;10(2):103–13. https://doi.org/10.1080/10640260290081678.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Dan-Glauser ES, Scherer KR. The Geneva affective picture database (GAPED): a new 730-picture database focusing on valence and normative significance. Behav Res Methods. 2011;43(2):468–77. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0064-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Kurdi B, Lozano S, Banaji MR. Introducing the open affective standardized image set (OASIS). Behav Res Methods. 2017;49(2):457–70. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0715-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Postzich C, Blask K, Frings C, Walther E. Timeless: a large sample study on the temporal robustness of affective responses. Front Psychol. 2016. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00841.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Tennant PWG, Arnold KF, Ellison GTH, Gilthorpe MS. Analyses of “change scores” do not estimate causal effects in observational data. Int J Epidemiol. 2022;51(5):1604–15. https://doi.org/10.1093/IJE/DYAB050.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows 28.0. 2021.

  44. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach. 3rd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 2022.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Delinsky SS, Wilson GT. Mirror exposure for the treatment of body image disturbance. Int J Eat Disord. 2006;39(2):108–16. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.20207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Zajonc RB. Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. J Pers Soc Psychol Monogr Suppl. 1968;9(2):1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Pook M, Brähler E, Tuschen-Caffier B. Figurunzufriedenheit von Frauen in Abhängigkeit vom Lebensalter: normative Daten für den Fragebogen zum Figurbewusstsein. Verhaltenstherapie. 2009;19(1):14–20. https://doi.org/10.1159/000203031.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A. The weirdest people in the world? Behav Brain Sci. 2010;33(2–3):61–83. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152X.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Halbeisen G, Brandt G, Paslakis G. A plea for diversity in eating disorders research. Front Psych. 2022;13(1):145. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.820043.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Olson KLL, Lillis J, Panza E, Wing RR, Quinn DM, Puhl RR. Body shape concerns across racial and ethnic groups among adults in the United States: more similarities than differences. Body Image. 2020;35:108. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BODYIM.2020.08.013.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Vansteenwegen D, Francken G, Vervliet B, De Clercq A, Eelen P. Resistance to extinction in evaluative conditioning. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2006;32(1):71–9. https://doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.32.1.71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The present work was performed by Katharina Dumstorf in fulfillment of the requirements for obtaining the degree “Dr. med.”. We would like to thank Stefanie Horndasch for providing part of the study materials. We acknowledge support by the Open Access Publication Funds of the Ruhr-Universität Bochum.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. This project was supported by the FoRUM Projektförderung of the Medical Faculty, Ruhr-University Bochum (F1026N-21), awarded to Georgios Paslakis.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

KD collected the data. KD and GH developed the methods, analyzed the data, and were major contributors in writing the manuscript. GH and GP provided supervision. All authors were involved in conceptualizing the project and read, edited, and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Georg Halbeisen.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Ethical approval and consent to participate

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, had been prospectively registered at https://aspredicted.org/FBB_Y3W, and the study protocol was reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Ruhr-University Bochum’s Medical Faculty at Campus East-Westphalia (application AZ 2021-791_1, February 4th, 2022). All participants gave informed consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1

. Indirect Effects of Pairings on Body Attractiveness Ratings.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Dumstorf, K., Halbeisen, G. & Paslakis, G. How evaluative pairings improve body dissatisfaction in adult women: evidence from a randomized-controlled online study. J Eat Disord 12, 18 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-024-00975-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-024-00975-4

Keywords