Skip to main content

Avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder differs from anorexia nervosa in delay discounting

Abstract

Background

Avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder (ARFID) and anorexia nervosa (AN) are the two primary restrictive eating disorders; however, they are driven by differing motives for inadequate dietary intake. Despite overlap in restrictive eating behaviors and subsequent malnutrition, it remains unknown if ARFID and AN also share commonalities in their cognitive profiles, with cognitive alterations being a key identifier of AN. Discounting the present value of future outcomes with increasing delay to their expected receipt represents a core cognitive process guiding human decision-making. A hallmark cognitive characteristic of individuals with AN (vs. healthy controls [HC]) is reduced discounting of future outcomes, resulting in reduced impulsivity and higher likelihood of favoring delayed gratification. Whether individuals with ARFID display a similar reduction in delay discounting as those with AN (vs. an opposing bias towards increased delay discounting or no bias) is important in informing transdiagnostic versus disorder-specific cognitive characteristics and optimizing future intervention strategies.

Method

To address this research question, 104 participants (ARFID: n = 57, AN: n = 28, HC: n = 19) completed a computerized Delay Discounting Task. Groups were compared by their delay discounting parameter (ln)k.

Results

Individuals with ARFID displayed a larger delay discounting parameter than those with AN, indicating steeper delay discounting (M ± SD = −6.10 ± 2.00 vs. −7.26 ± 1.73, p = 0.026 [age-adjusted], Hedges’ g = 0.59), with no difference from HC (p = 0.514, Hedges’ g = −0.35).

Conclusion

Our findings provide a first indication of distinct cognitive profiles among the two primary restrictive eating disorders. The present results, together with future research spanning additional cognitive domains and including larger and more diverse samples of individuals with ARFID (vs. AN), will contribute to identifying maintenance mechanisms that are unique to each disorder as well as contribute to the optimization and tailoring of treatment strategies across the spectrum of restrictive eating disorders.

Plain English Summary

Avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder (ARFID) and anorexia nervosa (AN) are both restrictive eating disorders. However, the reasons for restricting food intake differ between the two diagnoses. A key question in further understanding similarities and differences between ARFID and AN is to understand whether individuals with these disorders process information and make decisions in similar or distinct ways. When humans decide between two different outcomes (e.g., a smaller immediate or a larger delayed reward), outcomes decrease in their value the farther in the future we expect to receive them (delay discounting). Individuals with AN exhibit a reduced discounting of future outcomes, which makes them more likely to forego immediate gratification for later rewards. However, whether this holds true for individuals with ARFID too (or whether they show the opposite or no bias) is unknown. Our investigation is the first to compare delay discounting between individuals with ARFID, AN, and healthy controls (HC). Our results show that individuals with ARFID show more delay discounting than those with AN, with no difference from HC. Knowing how rewards are being chosen and decisions made (and knowing differences between diagnoses) will be helpful in further optimizing and tailoring treatments for restrictive eating disorders.

Background

Avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder (ARFID) is a psychological disorder defined by impaired growth/weight loss, nutritional deficiencies, dependence on tube feeding or nutritional supplements, and/or significant psychosocial impairment. While ARFID shares the key symptom of dietary restriction with anorexia nervosa (AN), individuals with ARFID do not describe shape and weight concerns as motives for dietary restriction. Rather, typical reasons for restriction in ARFID include sensitivity to sensory characteristics (e.g., taste, smell, texture) of food; fear of aversive consequences of eating (e.g., choking, vomiting, gastrointestinal pain); or lack of interest in eating or food.

Because cognitive profiles in AN are shown to relate to specific symptoms of the disorder, the similarities in disordered eating behaviors between ARFID and AN raise the question of whether the two conditions might also share commonalities in their cognitive profiles. Differences in cognitive functioning in AN compared to healthy controls (HC) are well-documented and considered to contribute to manifestation and maintenance of eating disorder pathology, including dietary restriction (e.g., [1, 2]). In contrast, whether/how cognitive functioning might be altered in individuals with ARFID is unknown, but important to study to inform diagnostic differences as well as possible mechanisms underlying reasons for restrictions—which may offer additional treatment targets.

Discounting the present value of future outcomes with increasing delay to their expected receipt represents a key cognitive process in guiding human decision-making, where different outcomes (e.g., rewards) with varying value and time lag to their attainment need to be weighed against each other to guide current behavior. The degree to the present value of a future outcome is discounted, and thus how future consequences of current actions are valued differs across individuals. In general, steeper discounting of the present value of a delayed outcome translates into a higher likelihood of choosing more immediate rewards over favoring long-term outcomes, and thus higher impulsivity. Conversely, less discounting results in a higher valuation of future action consequences, which has been associated with higher self-control [3, 4]. A hallmark cognitive characteristic of individuals with AN (vs. HC) is a reduced discounting of future outcomes and pronounced preference for future versus more immediate outcomes. When choosing between smaller, more immediate and larger, delayed monetary rewards, individuals with AN (compared to HC) display a stronger preference for delayed choices together with a lower delay discounting rate [3, 5, 6]. While documented independent of food, delay discounting is associated with the excessive self-control over food intake that is clinically observed in individuals with AN, in which individuals with AN often restrict consumption of highly palatable foods to support a long-term aim of excessive weight loss (e.g., [5, 7]). Moreover, in a prospective investigation following individuals with acute AN undergoing inpatient treatment to restore weight, differences in delay discounting (of monetary rewards) between individuals with AN and HC established pretreatment were no longer present following successful weight restoration [8].

Whether individuals with ARFID display a similar reduction in delay discounting as those with AN (vs. an opposing bias towards increased delay discounting or no bias) is unknown. Although food restriction in ARFID is not motivated by weight or shape concerns, individuals with ARFID similarly demonstrate difficulty eating—even when hungry—if preferred foods are not available. For example, individuals with sensory sensitivity or fear of aversive consequences as part of their ARFID presentation may resist immediate hunger and food cues for later, more desired or safer foods, and individuals with lack of interest may or may not find food rewarding at all. Thus, it is conceivable that those with ARFID may exhibit an ability to delay reward similar to what has been documented in those AN. On the other hand, clinical observation points towards people with ARFID potentially showing a preference for more short-term rewards. In line with this, a first investigation in children and adolescents with ARFID (age 6–18 years) showed lower behavioral inhibition compared to age-based norm values [9]. Unlike in AN, individuals with ARFID are not pursuing a long-term goal of controlling their shape or weight. Rather, food avoidance and restriction in ARFID seems to be characterized by inability to tolerate strong aversion or fear when it comes to food itself, with individuals instead opting for the immediate reward of avoidance. In the sensory sensitivity presentation, individuals describe avoiding tastes, textures, and smells that they experience as unpleasant or even disgusting. Individuals with a fear of aversive consequences presentation report attempts to avoid feared consequences of eating, such as choking, vomiting, or gastrointestinal pain. Finally, for those with a lack of interest presentation, it is common to experience a near or total lack of hunger cues, having a very low appetite, and feeling that eating is a chore, such that eating itself is unpleasant and preferably avoided. Across presentations, individuals with ARFID express that they wish to be able to eat a larger amount or variety of foods, but that their ARFID symptoms present an insurmountable barrier [10]. As such, it is conceivable that individuals with ARFID prefer the immediate rewarding effects of dietary avoidance/restriction compared to the delayed rewarding effects of expanding dietary intake and variety, resulting in steeper delay discounting and thus the opposing cognitive bias to the one observed in those with AN.

To the best of our knowledge, no study has examined delay discounting in individuals with ARFID, nor has any published work compared cognitive functioning in a sample with ARFID to that in a sample with AN—a comparison which is critical in order to clarify any potential relationship between dietary restriction and cognitive alterations in those with ARFID. Thus, the current study was designed to extend prior research by comparing delay discounting in a sample of individuals with ARFID, spanning both adolescents and young adults, to individuals with AN and to HC. Given that this is the first published work on delay discounting in adolescents and adults with ARFID, our primary aim was to investigate whether the ARFID group differed from the AN and HC groups. We tested this difference bi-directionally to include the possibility that individuals with ARFID could display a bias towards less pronounced discounting of future rewards (resembling the cognitive profile of AN as they resemble their restrictive eating behavior; Hypothesis #1) or a bias towards steeper discounting matching their increased impulsive behavior with increased impulsive choice (Hypothesis #2). Given that ARFID symptoms are heterogeneous across the three ARFID presentations, we additionally aimed to explore whether in the present study sample, ARFID presentation type (i.e., sensory sensitivity, fear of aversive consequences, lack of interest in eating or food) over and above the diagnosis of ARFID would be related to delay discounting rate.

Method

Participants

Table 1 provides demographic characteristics of the study sample.

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of the avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder (ARFID), anorexia nervosa (AN), and healthy control (HC) groups

ARFID and AN samples

We recruited treatment-seeking individuals with ARFID (n = 57) and AN (n = 28), ages 10–30 years from the Eating Disorders Clinical and Research Program, an outpatient eating disorders specialty clinic at Massachusetts General Hospital. To match our population of study, we purposefully included a broad age range in our sample. As an additional inclusion criterion, only individuals diagnosed with the restricting subtype of AN, as opposed to the binge-purge subtype, were considered for this investigation, given that binge eating and purging are not part of the pathology of ARFID and that we sought to compare across eating disorders where restriction is the primary symptom. Diagnoses were conferred during a routine clinical interview by each participant’s treating psychologist or psychiatrist according to DSM-5 criteria [11, 12]. For study participants with ARFID, clinicians additionally evaluated which of the three presentations (sensory sensitivity, fear of aversive consequences, and/or lack of interest in eating or food) the participant’s symptoms were most consistent with [13]. Clinicians could select more than one presentation for each participant if appropriate, given that ARFID presentations can co-occur [14, 15]. With regard to ARFID presentation, based on clinical interviews, 45 participants endorsed sensory sensitivity, 18 endorsed fear of aversive consequences, and 24 participants endorsed lack of interest in eating or food.

HC

We recruited HC (n = 19), ages 10–30 years to match the age range of the ARFID and AN groups, who had either served as healthy controls from an NIMH-funded study (R01MH108595) investigating the neurobiology of ARFID (n = 4), learned about the study from Rally (a Mass General Brigham-wide recruitment platform; n = 4), or participated via Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk, an online recruitment platform for survey research; n = 11). HC from the neurobiology study did not meet criteria for any current psychiatric disorder on the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia [16]. Adults and children from MTurk and Rally were classified as HC if they scored below clinical cut points of < 2.3 on the Eating Disorder Examination-Questionnaire [17], < 44 on the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory – Trait scale [18], < 16 on the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale [19], and < 10, 9, and 10, respectively, on the Picky Eating, Appetite, and Fear subscales of the Nine-Item ARFID Scale [13, 20]. To ensure data quality for individuals recruited through MTurk, survey settings prevented individuals from participating twice. Additionally and in line with recommendations for analyzing MTurk data [21], we ensured there were no duplicate entries by collecting MTurk worker IDs. To ensure the absence of “bots” or non-human workers in our dataset, we also embedded one or two validity checks in each measure (a practice which has been supported by Kung et al. [22]), later removing any MTurk participants who failed these validity checks [21]. Participant compensation varied by recruitment source. Clinic participants received no compensation as they completed the necessary measures as part of routine care. HC drawn from our team’s neurobiology of ARFID study received up to $300 (because the broader study involved two multi-hour visits, including not just questionnaires but also functional magnetic resonance imaging scans, test meals, and blood draws; the full protocol is outlined in [23]. MTurk and Rally participants received $1 for completing a pre-screener and $15 for completing a battery of questionnaires.

Measures

Demographics

All participants self-reported their age, sex, race, and ethnicity.

Delay Discounting Task

Participants completed a web-based Delay Discounting Task designed for self-administration via testmybrain.org [24]. In this task, participants made repeated hypothetical decisions about whether they preferred different amounts of money now or in the future, with both the size of the monetary reward and the length of time the participant would have to wait for the reward increasing gradually. The task included seven delay periods (two weeks, one month, six months, one year, three years, five years, and ten years). Participants completed six trials for each of the seven delay periods, plus four catch trials, for a total of 46 trials. Participants who scored ≤ 50% on catch trials were ultimately excluded from analyses (n = 1 participant was excluded after implementing this criterion, which did not alter our results). Trials began with choosing between US$500 now (present outcome option) and US$1,000 (future outcome option) after the delay period in question. Based on their response, the present outcome option was adjusted in consecutive trials using the following procedure: A change amount was calculated as 500/(2^(count-1)) with count representing the trial count for the respective delay period. If a participant selected the present outcome option, the present outcome option for the consecutive trial was set as current present outcome – change amount. If a participant chose the future outcome option, the present outcome option in the following trial was calculated as present outcome + change amount. The future outcome option was kept constant at US$1,000. Four catch trials with clearly preferred reward outcomes were included to confirm participants’ attention to the task. An individual’s delay discounting parameter k was calculated as an average across the seven series of choices, with higher scores indicating a steeper discounting of the value of delayed rewards.

Eating Disorder Examination-Questionnaire (EDE-Q)

The EDE-Q [17] is a well-established measure of pathological eating behaviors and related attitudes and cognitions. Participants rate the number of days over the past 28 days in which they engaged in behaviors. The EDE-Q generates four subscale scores, and a global score is calculated by taking the average of the subscale scores. In the current study, we used a cut-off score of 2.3 on the EDE-Q Global Score [25] to rule out eating disorders other than ARFID in the ARFID and HC groups. Internal consistency for the EDE-Q in our sample was excellent (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.96).

Data analysis

We conducted all analyses in R [26]. Packages used for analyses included: dplyr [27], psych [28], corrplot [29], tidyverse [30], ggplot2 [31], stringr [32], fmsb [33], rstatix [34]. To approximate normal distribution of k, k values were log-transformed using natural log prior to data analysis ([ln]k). Scores for parameter (ln)k are always negative, with higher (closer to 0) scores reflecting steeper discounting of the value of delayed rewards.

To test whether the ARFID, AN, and HC groups differ in their delay discounting, we compared group means of (ln)k using an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), with age as a covariate in all analyses. We then ran Bonferroni-corrected pairwise t-tests based on age-adjusted means to conduct planned comparisons with each group.

For the exploratory analysis of delay discounting within each ARFID presentation, we built a multiple linear regression model for the ARFID group regressing (ln)k on ARFID presentation type (sensory sensitivity, fear of aversive consequences, and lack of interest) and controlling for age. The three ARFID presentations were entered as dichotomous independent variables with responses coded as 0 or 1, where 1 represented having the presentation. We used multiple regression to account for overlap between presentations.

Results

Delay discounting in ARFID, AN, and HC groups

Group means and results for the hypotheses-testing ANCOVA are presented in Tables 2 and 3 and in Fig. 1. The ANCOVA showed a significant main effect of group on the delay discounting parameter (ln)k (F2, 101 = 5.93, p = 0.004, etap2 = 0.11). Bonferroni-corrected post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed that the ARFID group showed a higher delay discounting parameter (ln)k than the AN group (p = 0.026), while no difference between the ARFID and HC groups was observed (corrected p = 0.51). Finally, consistent with prior literature, the AN group scored significantly higher than HC (corrected p = 0.004; Fig. 1). A post hoc analysis of achieved power indicated that we had sufficient statistical power to test our hypotheses (etap2 = 0.11, 1-ß err prob = 0.88).

Table 2 Age-adjusted scores on the (log-transformed) delay discounting parameter (ln)k for the sample with avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder (ARFID), the sample with anorexia nervosa (AN), and the healthy control (HC) sample
Table 3 Results of Bonferroni-corrected pairwise t-tests for the (log-transformed) delay discounting parameter (ln)k and age between the ARFID, AN, and HC groups
Fig. 1
figure 1

Bar graph of age-adjusted group means and between-group Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) with planned pairwise comparisons for the (log-transformed) delay discounting parameter (ln)k. Note: Error bars represent SEM. ARFID = avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder; AN = anorexia nervosa; HC = healthy controls

Delay discounting across ARFID presentations

Our exploratory multiple linear regression model evaluating the contribution of ARFID presentation to delay discounting was not significant (F = 1.99, p = 0.109, adjusted R-squared = 0.07). In other words, in our sample, none of the ARFID presentations contributed significantly to the delay discounting parameter (ln)k (B = −1.37 [p = 0.068] for sensory sensitivity, B = −0.64 [p = 0.321] for fear of aversive consequences, and B = −0.95 [p = 0.073] for lack of interest).

Discussion

Alterations in preference for delayed reward are a hallmark of AN. The current research is the first to examine whether differences in delay discounting might exist in adults and children with ARFID compared to those with AN and to HC. Consistent with our Hypothesis #2, we found that those with ARFID exhibited a steeper discounting of future monetary outcomes (translating into a higher likelihood of choosing more immediate outcomes) than those with AN (with no difference to HC). This finding fits previous clinical observations. More specifically, individuals with AN may exert cognitive control to reduce food intake, thus delaying reward (i.e., food) notably compared to healthy individuals. On the other hand, those with ARFID may find food in itself less rewarding, and thus may not experience a need to utilize cognitive control resources to delay/prevent dietary intake. In other words, these findings indicate that the individual’s motivation for restriction may matter significantly in understanding disorder persistence, over and above the restriction behaviors themselves. Another possible interpretation of our findings is that preference for more immediate reward may even offer an explanation for some of the challenges many individuals with ARFID seem to face: namely, opting for the short-term reward of safe foods and avoiding negative feelings over the long-term reward of expanding dietary variety or volume. Additionally, while we did not observe a statistically significant difference in delay discounting between those with ARFID and healthy individuals, the large difference in mean scores between the two groups may be a signal to investigate other approaches—for example, examining delay discounting with severity of ARFID symptoms rather than the binary outcome of having ARFID or not. Future research following this signal (e.g., investigating delay discounting as a mediator between avoidance behaviors and disorder persistence) and attempting to further understand the possible role of delay discounting in the maintenance of ARFID is warranted.

The current research is a compelling introduction to a distinct cognitive profile of ARFID on which future research can expand. This research offers novel findings as it is the first to examine delay discounting in individuals with ARFID. We used a well-supported measure of this cognitive construct (i.e., the testmybrain.org Delay Discounting Task), which is important considering the task impurity problem common in cognitive and neuropsychological testing [1, 24]. Prior research suggests that discounting for hypothetical and real rewards produces very similar results, supporting the validity of the testmybrain.org Delay Discounting Task [35]. To date, only one published study has explored aspects of cognitive functioning in youth with ARFID [9], and to our knowledge, no prior studies have explored aspects of cognitive functioning in adults with ARFID or characterized delay discounting in any ARFID sample. The current study indicates that those with ARFID may have a cognitive profile that is distinct from those with AN. Understanding similarities and differences in the cognitive profile underlying ARFID compared to that in AN may contribute to the pursuit of improved clinical care in ARFID, given current conceptualizations of ARFID as cognitive-behavioral in nature [36]. Gaining an increased understanding of how cognition might be similar or different in those with ARFID compared to those with AN and other eating disorders treated with cognitively-oriented treatments (i.e., cognitive-behavioral therapy) may elucidate cognition treatment targets. For example, those with ARFID might exhibit some cognitive features that are similar to what is seen in AN, such as cognitive rigidity [37, 38] or all-or-nothing thinking [39]. Thinking of this nature has been observed in individuals with ARFID in an outpatient setting [10]. These and other potential cognitive similarities and differences between those with ARFID and those with AN provide a compelling avenue for future research.

Limitations of the current research should be recognized. Generalizability is a potential limitation of this research, given that our measure of delay discounting utilized monetary rewards; future research should seek to replicate these findings with food rewards, to investigate the possibility that individuals with ARFID may respond differently to rewards that are more salient to their disorder. While the overall sample was moderate in size, some presentations within the ARFID group (though only considered in a follow-up exploratory analysis) contained a relatively small number of participants (i.e., n = 18 for fear of aversive consequences). Further, the HC group was small due to cleaning measures applied to the data (e.g., applying an age cutoff to match the age range of the ARFID and AN groups). Replication in samples with greater numbers of both participants with ARFID and healthy control participants is needed to corroborate our findings. While all individuals in the HC group were equivalently screened for eligibility, we employed a heterogeneous sample recruited from multiple sources. Low racial and ethnic diversity bears on the generalizability of the study to diverse populations, given that these aspects of cognitive processing are known to vary between different populations and cultural contexts (e.g., [40,41,42,43,44,45]). Additionally, while we controlled for age in the present study to account for the wide developmental range of our sample (ages 10–30), developmental age is an important consideration for future research.

Conclusions

Future research should aim to expand on this promising initial finding by investigating potential relationships between delay discounting and severity of ARFID symptoms as well as examining these constructs longitudinally. Further, there are many other domains of cognitive functioning that may be worthwhile to examine together in samples with ARFID to evaluate the full cognitive profile. Understanding similarities and differences in the cognitive mechanisms underlying ARFID compared to AN may help elucidate new maintenance mechanisms for this condition.

Availability of data and materials

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.

Abbreviations

ARFID:

Avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder

AN:

Anorexia nervosa

HC:

Healthy control

Mturk:

Amazon Mechanical Turk

EDE-Q:

Eating Disorder Examination-Questionnaire

ANCOVA:

Analysis of covariance

References

  1. Dann KM, Hay P, Touyz S. Are poor set-shifting and central coherence associated with everyday function in anorexia nervosa? A systematic review. J Eat Disord. 2021;9(1):40. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-021-00392-x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Schmidt U, Treasure J. Anorexia nervosa: valued and visible. A cognitive-interpersonal maintenance model and its implications for research and practice. Br J Clin Psychol. 2006;45(Pt 3):343–66. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466505x53902.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Amlung M, Marsden E, Holshausen K, Morris V, Patel H, Vedelago L, Naish KR, Reed DD, McCabe RE. Delay discounting as a transdiagnostic process in psychiatric disorders: a meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiat. 2019;76(11):1176–86. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.2102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Rachlin H. The science of self-control. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Steinglass JE, Figner B, Berkowitz S, Simpson HB, Weber EU, Walsh BT. Increased capacity to delay reward in anorexia nervosa. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012;18(4):773–80. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617712000446.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Steinglass JE, Lempert KM, Choo TH, Kimeldorf MB, Wall M, Walsh BT, Simpson HB. Temporal discounting across three psychiatric disorders: Anorexia nervosa, obsessive compulsive disorder, and social anxiety disorder. Depress Anxiety. 2017;34(5):463–70. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22586.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Steward T, Mestre-Bach G, Vintró-Alcaraz C, Agüera Z, Jiménez-Murcia S, Granero R, Fernández-Aranda F. Delay discounting of reward and impulsivity in eating disorders: from anorexia nervosa to binge eating disorder. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2017;25(6):601–6. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2543.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Decker JH, Figner B, Steinglass JE. On weight and waiting: delay discounting in anorexia nervosa pretreatment and posttreatment. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;78(9):606–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.12.016.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Basile C, Gigliotti F, Colaiori M, Santo FD, Terrinoni A, Ardizzone I, Sabatello U. Comparison of neuropsychological profiles in children and adolescent with anorexia nervosa and avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder (ARFID). Eur Psychiatry. 2021;64(S1):S351–2. https://doi.org/10.1192/j.eurpsy.2021.942.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Thomas JJ, Becker KR, Eddy KT. The picky eater’s recovery book. Cambridge University Press; 2021.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  11. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. American Psychiatric Association; 2013.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  12. Becker KR, Keshishian AC, Liebman RE, Coniglio KA, Wang SB, Franko DL, Eddy KT, Thomas JJ. Impact of expanded diagnostic criteria for avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder on clinical comparisons with anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord. 2019;52(3):230–8. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22988.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Burton Murray H, Dreier MJ, Zickgraf HF, Becker KR, Breithaupt L, Eddy KT, Thomas JJ. Validation of the nine item ARFID screen (NIAS) subscales for distinguishing ARFID presentations and screening for ARFID. Int J Eat Disord. 2021;54(10):1782–92. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.23520.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Reilly EE, Brown TA, Gray EK, Kaye WH, Menzel JE. Exploring the Cooccurrence of behavioural phenotypes for avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder in a partial hospitalization sample. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2019;27(4):429–35. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2670.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Thomas JJ, Lawson EA, Micali N, Misra M, Deckersbach T, Eddy KT. Avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder: a three-dimensional model of neurobiology with implications for etiology and treatment. In: Current Psychiatry Reports (Vol. 19, Issue 8, pp. 1–9). Current Medicine Group LLC 1; 2017. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-017-0795-5

  16. Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Axelson D, Perepletchikova F, Brent D, Ryan N. K-SADS-PL DSM-5 November 2016 (2016). Retrieved from https://www.pediatricbipolar.pitt.edu/sites/default/files/KSADS_DSM_5_SCREEN_Final.pdf.

  17. Fairburn C, Beglin S. Eating disorder examination questionnaire. Cogn Behav Ther Eat Disord. 2008;309:313.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Spielberger C, Gorsuch R, Lushene R, Vagg P, Jacobs G. Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press; 1983.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population; 1977.

  20. Zickgraf HF, Ellis JM. Initial validation of the Nine Item Avoidant/Restrictive Food Intake disorder screen (NIAS): a measure of three restrictive eating patterns. Appetite. 2018;123:32–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2017.11.111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Kambanis PE, Bottera AR, de Young KP. Eating disorder prevalence among Amazon MTurk workers assessed using a rigorous online, self-report anthropometric assessment. Eat Behav. 2021;41: 101481. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.EATBEH.2021.101481.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Kung FY, Kwok N, Brown DJ. Are attention check questions a threat to scale validity? Appl Psychol. 2018;67(2):264–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Burton Murray H, Becker KR, Harshman S, Breithaupt L, Kuhnle M, Dreier MJ, Hauser K, Freizinger M, Eddy KT, Misra M, Kuo B, Micali N, Thomas JJ, Lawson EA. Elevated fasting satiety-promoting cholecystokinin (CCK) in avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder compared to healthy controls. J Clin Psychiatry. 2022;83(5):14111. https://doi.org/10.4088/jcp.21m14111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Germine LT, Joormann J, Passell E, Rutter LA, Scheuer L, Martini P, Hwang I, Lee S, Sampson N, Barch DM, House SL, Beaudoin FL, An X, Stevens JS, Zeng D, Linnstaedt SD, Jovanovic T, Clifford GD, Neylan TC, Kessler RC. Neurocognition after motor vehicle collision and adverse post-traumatic neuropsychiatric sequelae within 8 weeks: initial findings from the AURORA study. J Affect Disord. 2022;298:57–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2021.10.104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Mond JM, Hay PJ, Rodgers B, Owen C, Beumont PJV. Validity of the Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire (EDE-Q) in screening for eating disorders in community samples. Behav Res Ther. 2004;42(5):551–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00161-X.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing (2018). Available from https://www.r-project.org/.

  27. Wickham H, François R, Henry L, Müller K. dplyr: A Grammar of Data Manipulation (2022). https://dplyr.tidyverse.org, https://github.com/tidyverse/dplyr.

  28. Revelle W. psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research. Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois. R package version 2.2.9 (2022). https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych.

  29. Wei T, Simko V. R package 'corrplot': Visualization of a Correlation Matrix. (Version 0.92) (2021). https://github.com/taiyun/corrplot.

  30. Wickham H, Averick M, Bryan J, Chang W, McGowan LD, François R, Grolemund G, Hayes A, Henry L, Hester J, Kuhn M, Pedersen TL, Miller E, Bache SM, Müller K, Ooms J, Robinson D, Seidel DP, Spinu V, Takahashi K, Vaughan D, Wilke C, Woo K, Yutani H. Welcome to the tidyverse. J Open Source Softw. 2019;4(43):1686. https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.01686.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Wickham H. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. Springer New York; 2016. ISBN 978-3-319-24277-4, https://ggplot2.tidyverse.org.

  32. Wickham H. stringr: Simple, Consistent Wrappers for Common String Operations (2022). https://stringr.tidyverse.org, https://github.com/tidyverse/stringr.

  33. Nakazawa M. fmsb: Functions for medical statistics book with some demographic data (2022). Available from https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/fmsb/index.html.

  34. Kassambara A. Pipe-friendly framework for basic statistical tests [R package rstatix version 0.7.1]. The Comprehensive R Archive Network (2022). Retrieved January 29, 2023, from https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/rstatix/index.html

  35. Lawyer SR, Schoepflin F, Green R, Jenks C. Discounting of hypothetical and potentially real outcomes in nicotine-dependent and nondependent samples. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2011;19(4):263–74. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Thomas JJ, Eddy KT. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder: children, adolescents, & adults. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2019.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Tchanturia K, Campbell IC, Morris R, Treasure J. Neuropsychological studies in anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord. 2005;37(Suppl):S72–89. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.20119.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Tchanturia K, Davies H, Roberts M, Harrison A, Nakazato M, Schmidt U, Morris R. Poor cognitive flexibility in eating disorders: examining the evidence using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(1): e28331.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Garner DM, Bemis KM. A cognitive-behavioral approach to anorexia nervosa. Cogn Ther Res. 1982;6:123–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Egbert A, Hunt RA, Williams K, Burke NL, Jennings MK. Reporting racial and ethnic diversity in eating disorder research over the past 20 years. Int J Eat Disord. 2021;55:455–62. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/ys6xz.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Goel NJ, Jennings Mathis K, Egbert AH, Petterway F, Breithaupt L, Eddy KT, Franko DL, Graham AK. Accountability in promoting representation of historically marginalized racial and ethnic populations in the eating disorders field: a call to action. Int J Eat Disord. 2022;55(4):463–9. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.23682.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Ishii K, Eisen C, Hitokoto H. The effects of social status and culture on delay discounting. Jpn Psychol Res. 2017;59(3):230–7. https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Kim B, Sung YS, McClure SM. The neural basis of cultural differences in delay discounting. Philos Trans R Soc B: Biol Sci. 2012;367(1589):650–6. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Lao J, Vizioli L, Caldara R. Culture modulates the temporal dynamics of global/local processing. Cult Brain. 2013;1(2–4):158–74. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40167-013-0012-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Oishi S, Jaswal VK, Lillard AS, Mizokawa A, Hitokoto H, Tsutsui Y. Cultural variations in global versus local processing: a developmental perspective. Dev Psychol. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038272.supp.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

Research reported in this publication was supported by the Charles A. King Trust (FP), the National Institutes of Health: National Institute of Mental Health R01MH108595 (JJT), R01MH103402 (KTE), R03MH103402 (LB), KL2TR002542 (LB), F31MH125495 (SBW), and K23MH125143 (KRB), and the National Science Foundation (SBW, DGE-1745303). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the funding agencies.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

CMS: Investigation, Methodology, Data curation, Formal analysis, Visualization, Writing—original draft, Writing—review & editing. IM: Writing—original draft, Writing—review & editing. MJD: Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing—review & editing. KC: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing. LLP: Formal analysis, Writing—review & editing. JG: Data curation, Writing—review & editing. HG: Writing—review & editing. LTG: Formal analysis, Writing—review & editing. NT: Formal analysis, Writing—review & editing. SBW: Writing—review & editing. LB: Data curation, Writing—review & editing. KTE: Data curation, Writing—review & editing. JJT: Supervision, Data curation, Writing—original draft, Writing—review & editing. FP: Supervision, Conceptualization, Methodology, Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing—original draft, Writing—review & editing. KRB: Supervision, Conceptualization, Methodology, Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing—original draft, Writing—review & editing.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kendra R. Becker.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki and study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board (Protocol 2013P002614). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Consent for publication

The authors consent for this manuscript to be published. There are no other parties involved in the decision to publish.

Competing interests

Drs. Becker, Eddy, and Thomas receive royalties from Cambridge University Press for the sale of their books on ARFID. Dr. Breithaupt is a consultant for Otsuka Pharmaceuticals. Dr. Thomas receives royalties for book sales from Oxford University Press for her forthcoming book on rumination syndrome.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Stern, C.M., McPherson, I., Dreier, M.J. et al. Avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder differs from anorexia nervosa in delay discounting. J Eat Disord 12, 19 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-023-00958-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-023-00958-x

Keywords