Skip to main content

Perfectionism and binge eating association: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Abstract

Background

Perfectionism is considered a vulnerability factor for eating disorders. However, the role of perfectionism in binge eating needs clarification due to notably inconsistencies between studies. The purpose to this study was to conduct a systematic review and meta-analysis to estimate the perfectionism-binge eating association.

Method

Systematic review was performed according to the PRISMA 2020 statement. Four databases (Web of Science, Scopus, PsycINFO and Psicodoc) were searched to identify studies published until September 2022. The literature search yielded 30 published articles (N = 9392) that provided 33 independent estimations of the correlation between the two variables.

Results

Random-effects meta-analysis revealed a small-to-moderate positive average effect size between general perfectionism and binge eating (r+ = .17) with a large heterogeneity. Perfectionistic Concerns showed a significant small-to-moderate relationship with binge eating (r+ = .27), whereas Perfectionistic Strivings presented a negligible relationship with binge eating (r+ = .07). Moderator analyses showed that the age, the type of the sample, the study design, and the tools for assessing both variables were statistically associated with the perfectionism-binge eating effect sizes.

Conclusions

Our findings suggest that Perfectionism Concerns are closely associated with binge eating symptomatology. This relationship might be moderated by certain variables, especially by the clinical or non-clinical nature of the sample and the instrument employed to assess binge eating.

Plain English summary

Perfectionism is a trait of personality comprising two facets, Perfectionistic Strivings (entails the desire to reach perfection and to pursue unrealistically high standards) and Perfectionistic Concerns (involves self-criticism, concerns over making mistakes, fears about social negative evaluation and lack of satisfaction with achievements). Perfectionist individuals have an increased risk for developing eating disorders. However, whether perfectionism or any of its facets is associated with binge eating (an episode of overeating together with a feeling of loss of control) is an unanswered question. The purpose of this systematic review and meta-analysis was to clarify this question. Our results evidenced that overall perfectionism is associated with binge eating. It means that perfectionist people are more vulnerable to developing binge eating symptomatology, although Perfectionistic Concerns entails a higher risk in comparison with Perfectionistic Strivings. Our study also provides valuable information on the aspects that might explain the variations in the results of previous studies that have analyzed the perfectionism-binge eating association.

Background

Binge eating is defined as “consuming abnormally large quantities of food in a discrete time period with a concurrent sense of loss of control” [2]. Considered as a transdiagnostic symptom, binge eating occurs across different eating disorders such as Binge Eating Disorder, Bulimia Nervosa, and the binge/purge subtype of Anorexia Nervosa [1]. Although central to the diagnosis of eating disorders, recurrent binge eating is also associated with both, physical and mental health problems, such as overweight or obesity, chronic pain, diabetes, hypertension, anxiety, and depression, as well as with a poor psychosocial functioning, including lower quality of life [16, 48]. Not limited to clinical population, this deviant behavior is considered a subthreshold symptom [101] with a considerable prevalence in general community, affecting 4.9% of females and 4% of males over lifetimes (see [107], for a review). Hence, binge eating represents a significant public health challenge [104], not only because of its considerable incidence, but also due to its numerous adverse consequences. Unfortunately, binge eating, as a specific symptom, remains underresearched [38].

Perfectionism, in contrast, has received a great deal of attention from research. This trait of personality is conceptualized as multidimensional, and it includes several intrapersonal and interpersonal facets [49]. There is an agreement about the existence of two higher-order perfectionism factors: Perfectionistic Strivings and Perfectionistic Concerns. This two-factor structure provides an empirical and theoretical background to compare results obtained by using different perfectionism scales and subscales [99]. Perfectionistic Strivings is considered the adaptive, or at least not maladaptive, dimension of perfectionism, and it entails the desire to reach perfection and to pursue unrealistically high standards. In contrast, Perfectionistic Concerns reflects aspects associated with self-criticism, concerns over making mistakes, fears about social negative evaluation and lack of satisfaction with achievements, representing a maladaptive component [100]. The study of perfectionism is of a huge relevance, because this trait seems to be implicated in the genesis, development, and maintenance of a wide range of psychopathologies [60]. In particular, perfectionism has long been associated to eating disorders [6]. However, while the link with Anorexia and Bulimia Nervosa is well-stablished, the association between perfectionism and Binge Eating Disorder is less clear and more research is needed to explain inconsistencies [22, 30, 57].

Perfectionism and binge eating

Perfectionism is a core variable included across several models addressing the maintenance and persistence of binge eating behaviors (see [73], for a review). Among all these models, the Perfectionism Model of Binge Eating (PMOBE) is the only one that explain the why and how perfectionism is related with binge eating. According to this formulation, Socially Prescribed Perfectionism (a dimension described by Hewitt and Flett [49], that captures beliefs about perfectionistic demands and criticisms from significant others) promotes vulnerability to binge eating by increasing interpersonal discrepancies, low interpersonal esteem, depressive affect, and dietary restraint [85, 87]. Mackinnon et al. [65] purposed a reformulation of the model focusing on another perfectionism dimension, i.e., Concern Over Mistakes. This dimension entails the negative reactions to mistakes, interpreting them in terms of failure, and the tendency to believe that others can stop respecting us because of our mistakes [39]. Both, Socially Prescribed Perfectionism and Concern Over Mistakes are considered indicators of Perfectionistic Concerns [100]. Therefore, taken together the original and reformulated PMOBE, it seems that Perfectionistic Concerns is relevant to the occurrence of binge eating. More recently, the authors of the original PMOBE also highlighted the role of mother-daughter relationship in both perfectionism and binge eating [58]. Specifically, they evidenced that mother’s perfectionistic concerns would raise the risk of binge eating for themselves and their daughters.

Unfortunately, although there is some evidence for the PMOBE, additional direct empirical test, particularly with clinical samples, is still being necessary [73]. Moreover, the model also fails in clarifying the role of other indicators of Perfectionistic Concerns different from Socially Prescribed Perfectionism and Concern Over Mistakes, in the development of binge eating. For example, research has shown that Doubts About Actions (understood as the tendency to believe that projects are not completed to satisfaction, [39] is consistently linked with binge eating behaviors [64, 88, 89, 92]. Therefore, is the Perfectionistic Concerns second-order factor which best explains the relationship between perfectionism and binge eating, or are other specific indicators (i.e., Socially Prescribed Perfectionism, Doubts About Actions, and Concern Over Mistakes) more closely related to it?

Regarding Perfectionistic Strivings, neither the original nor the reformulated PMOBE included this dimension in the explanation of binge eating behavior. This is because results more strongly implicated Perfectionistic Concerns in comparison with Perfectionistic Strivings in binge eating. However, Mackinnon et al. [65] warned about the necessity for further research to clarify a possible role of Perfectionistic Strivings in binge eating. Certainly, the relationship between these two variables is unclear due to inconsistencies between studies’ findings. Indeed, whereas some studies have informed about a positive and statistically significant association between Perfectionistic Strivings and binge eating (e.g., [7, 11, 65, 68, 70, 85]), others reported a non-significant relationship, [5, 67, 106]. These discrepancies can be explained by different reasons, such as the dimension employed as an indicator of Perfectionistic Strivings, the sample size and type (i.e., community vs. clinical), the design of the study, etc.

Finally, it is important to notice that there are other forms of perfectionism, beyond the indicators of Perfectionistic Concerns and Perfectionistic Strivings, which might have an important explanatory role on being eating. For example, empirical studies have reported positive and significant correlations between binge eating and: Perfectionistic Self-Promotion [10, 71], a form of perfectionistic self-presentation characterized by the need to actively display one’s own perfection to others [51],self-criticism [88, 108], and parental criticisms [64] and expectations [70]. In contrast, when perfectionism is assessed as unidimensional by using a subset of items of the Bulimia subscale of the Eating Disorder Inventory (EDI, [41]) or its reviewed (EDI-2; [40]) and children versions (EDI-C; [102], the association with binge eating is unclear,some studies found a positive and significant relationship (e.g., [55, 69, 70] whereas others reported non-significant correlations (e.g., [24, 46, 80]). Hence, these noteworthy inconsistences have darkened our understanding of that relationship.

This study

From our knowledge, no one has yet systematically studied the association between perfectionism and binge eating, as a specific symptom, using a meta-analysis. A prior meta-analysis performed by Kehayes et al. [57] with 12 longitudinal studies examined whether perfectionism would predict increased bulimic symptoms (i.e., binge eating and compensatory behaviors). Unfortunately, authors did not perform independent analysis for each bulimic symptomatology. Hence, it was not possible to draw a conclusion about the relationship between perfectionism and binge eating. As mentioned above, despite the broadly reported data of the literature supporting the association between perfectionism and eating pathology [6], the role of this personality trait in explaining binge eating behavior is not clear yet. Although previous research evidenced a positive and significant relationship between Perfectionistic Concerns and binge eating, a meta-analysis could allow a deeper understanding of this relationship by providing data about which indicator or dimension of Perfectionistic Concerns exhibit the strongest relations with binge eating. Moreover, because previous research disagrees on whether the relationship between Perfectionistic Strivings and binge eating is significant or not, a meta-analysis could help clarify this disagreement. In addition, a meta-analysis might allow to identify moderating variables that could explain those variations in results of previous studies. Last but not least, a quantitative synthesis is also needed for shedding light on the association between other forms of perfectionism and binge eating.

Therefore, the present study aimed to provide the first meta-analysis of research examining the relationship between perfectionism and binge eating. Specifically, the mean effect sizes for the relationship between binge eating and perfectionism were estimated, not only for Perfectionistic Concerns and Perfectionistic Strivings, but also for each specific indicator of these two higher-order dimensions, as well as for other forms of perfectionism. Secondly, the effects of potential moderators (i.e., the type of the sample, gender, age, nationality of the sample or measurement instruments for perfectionism and binge eating, among others), that might explain the heterogeneous outcomes were also examined.

Method

This systematic review and meta-analysis was conducted considering the PRISMA 2020 guidelines [76].

Eligibility criteria

Studies had to fulfil the following criteria: (a) original and quantitative investigations, (b) published in English or Spanish, (c) containing data on perfectionism and binge eating measured with validated tools, and (d) providing at least a correlation coefficient for the relationship between these two variables.

Searching for the studies and selection process

The search was conducted on September 2022 in the databases: Web of Science, Scopus, PsycINFO and Psicodoc, using the strategy perfectionis* AND binge. Since no time limitation was established, the search period covered any document published in these databases until September 25, 2022. The search in PsycINFO and Psicodoc was performed at the same time. This is because we accessed these two databases through EBSCOhost and it was available the possibility to search on both simultaneously. We refined results to exclude documents written in any language different from English or Spanish.

Figure 1 shows a PRISMA flow diagram of the study screening and selection process. A total of 594 references were obtained from the three databases. After eliminating duplicates, we looked for the full texts of the remaining records. Of the 339 full-text documents analyzed, 127 were excluded because they did not assess perfectionism or binge eating, and 97 because data was not enough (i.e., documents did not provide a correlation coefficient). Therefore, the final sample for the meta-analysis was composed of 30 studies and 33 independent samples.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flowchart of the study selection process in the systematic review and meta-analysis of Perfectionism and Binge Eating

Coding of studies

The data extracted from each study were: publication year, sample type, percentage of female participants, mean age and standard deviation of participants, country where the study was conducted, study design (longitudinal or cross-sectional), measurement tools employed to assess perfectionism and binge eating, and the correlation coefficients reported for the association between binge eating and perfectionism. In order to assess the reliability of the data extraction process, two raters independently extracted the information. Inconsistencies were resolved by consensus. Concordance between the two raters was assessed by calculating Cohen’s kappa coefficients, achieving a satisfactory average value of .96. The data that supports the findings of this study are available in the supplementary material of this article (see Additional file 1).

Meta-analytic procedures

The effect size index in this meta-analysis was the correlation coefficient between perfectionism and binge eating measurements. From each study, all correlation coefficients reported between these two constructs were extracted. In order to normalize its distribution and stabilize the variances, the correlation coefficients were transformed into Fisher’s Z.

To avoid dependency problems, separate meta-analyses were conducted for each combination of perfectionism and binge eating measurements. In addition, correlation coefficients obtained from the same sample were averaged to conduct a global meta-analysis avoiding dependency problems. In each meta-analysis a random-effects model was assumed, as it was expected heterogeneity among the correlation coefficients. Thus, each effect size was weighted by its inverse-variance, this defined as the sum of the sampling variance and the between-studies variance. The between-studies variance was estimated by restricted maximum likelihood [13, 17]. For each meta-analysis, a forest plot was constructed and an average effect size with a 95% confidence interval with the improved method proposed by Hartung and Knapp [83]. Heterogeneity was assessed with the Q statistic and the I2 index. Publication bias was assessed by constructing funnel plots and by applying the Egger test and the trim-and-fill method for imputing missing effect sizes [82]. Fisher’s Zs were back-transformed to the correlation coefficient metric in order to make easy their interpretation. The influence of moderator variables was assessed by means of subgroup analyses (ANOVAs) for categorical moderators and by means of meta-regression models for the continuous ones. In all cases, the statistical significance of each moderator was tested with the improved F-statistic proposed by Knapp and Hartung [103]. The proportion of variance explained by each moderator was estimated with the R2 index [62]. Moderator analyses were conducted only for meta-analyses with at least 30 effect sizes. This only happened for the global correlation coefficients obtained from each of the 33 independent samples. All statistical analyses were carried out with the program Comprehensive Meta-analysis 3.3 [14].

Results

Description of studies

The main characteristics of the studies are presented in Appendix 1. Relevant data were obtained from 29 journal articles and one dissertation. Three studies provided data for two separate samples [5, 90, 105]. The total number of participants pooled across studies was 9392. Sample size ranged from 89 to 566. Of the 33 samples included in this meta-analysis, ten were clinical and 23 non-clinical. Most of the non-clinical samples were composed by undergraduates (k = 16). There were also three samples composed by adolescents or high school students, two samples of twins, one sample of emerging adults, and another of adults. Regarding the studies with clinical samples, they included patients with an eating disorder, and/or overweight or obesity. The average percentage of female participants was 82%. Twenty of the 33 samples were composed only by females, whereas three samples were exclusively composed by males. The mean age of participants through the studies was 21.6 (SD = 4.1; range = 11.5–41.8). The majority of the samples (k = 28) were conducted in North American countries (i.e., Canada and USA). Of the 30 studies, 18 used a cross-sectional design and five a longitudinal one, whereas the remaining seven studies provided both longitudinal and cross-sectional data.

The studies used a variety of scales to assess perfectionism. Following Stoeber [99]’s classification, we considered the following subscales as indicators of Perfectionistic Concerns: Concern Over Mistakes, Doubts About Actions (Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale, FMPS, [39], Socially Prescribed Perfectionism (Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale, HMPS, [49], Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale-Short Form, SF-HMPS; [52], Reactivity to Mistakes (Measure of Constructs Underlying Perfectionism, M-CUP, [94], and Perfectionistic Discrepancies (Resconstructed Depressive Experiencies Questionnaire, DEQ-R; [3], Almost Perfect Scale-Revised, APS-R, [91], Multidimensional Discrepancy Inventory, MDI; Flett and Hewitt [31]). On the other hand, Personal Standards (FMPS; [39], SF-FMPS; [21] and Self-Oriented Perfectionism (HMPS [49], SF-HMPS; [52] subscales were considered as indicators of Perfectionistic Strivings. Some studies tended to combine more than one measure of perfectionism to assess these two-higher order dimensions. Other forms of perfectionism, such as Other-Oriented Perfectionism (HMPS [49], SF-HMPS; [52], Parental Criticisms, Parental Expectations (FMPS, [39], Perfectionistic Self-Promotion (Perfectionistic Self-Presentation Scale, PSPS, [51], Self-criticism (Depressive Experiencies Questionnaire, DEQ, [8],Self-Rating Scale, SRS; [54],Self-Criticism/Self-Reassurance Scale, FSC; [42], and the Perfectionism subscale from the EDI [41] and its different versions (EDI-2, [40], EDI-C; [102] were excluded from this classification. It is important to notice that, although the Perfectionism subscale of the EDI is a unidimensional measure of perfectionism, a two-dimensional structure representing Self-Oriented Perfectionism and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism was identified [86]. Consequently, when studies considered a two-dimensional structure (this was the case of Lampard et al. [59], and [85], each subscale, Self-Oriented Perfectionism and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism, was considered, respectively, as an indicator of Perfectionistic Strivings and Perfectionistic Concerns. Figure 2 shows a classification of the measures employed by the studies included in the meta-analysis as indicators of Perfectionistic Concerns, Perfectionistic Strivings, and other forms of perfectionism.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Subscales (and scales) used as indicators of Perfectionistic Concerns and Perfectionistic Strivings, as well as other forms of perfectionism

Binge eating symptoms were assessed through different tools. Two studies assessed the frequency of binge eating behavior by using the Eating Disorders Examination (EDE; [27]), an interview widely used to assess eating disorder symptoms. In addition, in Jones and Crowther’s [55] study binge eating was assessed by asking the participants whether they engaged or not in binging during the previous year. Apart from these three investigations, studies included in this meta-analysis used self-report measures to assess binge eating tendencies. The EDE-Q [25], which is the self-report form of the EDE, and recent versions of that questionnaire, such as the EDE-Q 6.0 [26] and the version for children (ChEDE-Q, [53]), were the most used measures to assess binge eating (k = 9). Another measure widely employed was the Bulimia subscale of the EDI [41] and EDI-2 [40] (k = 7). As that subscale assesses symptoms of bulimia, purging and binge eating, authors employed different subsets of the items focused on binge eating. Other examples of self-report scales employed were a 7-item version of the Binge Eating subscale from the Eating Disorder Diagnostic Scale (EDDS; [97] (k = 3), and the Binge Eating Scale (BES; [45]) (k = 2), between others.

Average effect size and heterogeneity

The association between perfectionism and binge eating was estimated in this meta-analysis. Figure 3 presents a forest plot of the correlations found between both variables in each individual study and their 95% CI for the 33 independent samples. The forest plot also presents the average correlation coefficient resulting from pooling all the studies, reaching a value of r+ = .17 (95% CI = .12–.22, k = 33). To help to interpret the correlation coefficient magnitude, we followed Cohen’s [19] guidelines for small, medium, and large effects (r = .10, .30, and .50, respectively). Thus, the average global correlation coefficient between perfectionism and binge eating exhibited a positive, small-to-moderate magnitude. The individual effect sizes exhibited a large heterogeneity, Q(32) = 196.54, p < .001, I2 = 83.7% (Table 1).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Forest plot of the association between Perfectionism and Binge Eating for the global correlation coefficients obtained from the 33 independent samples

Table 1 Summary of overall effect sizes for the relationship between perfectionism dimensions and binge eating

Separate meta-analyses were also conducted to examine the associations between perfectionism and binge eating but establishing differences between the two higher-order factors of perfectionism (i.e., Perfectionistic Concerns and Perfectionistic Strivings) as well as for each specific perfectionism dimension. Because the data about the relationship between some perfectionism dimensions and binge eating were underrepresented, a minimum of studies (k ≥ 3 independent studies) was stablished to calculate effect sizes for each specific dimension. However, those data were considered for the analyses of their respective higher-order dimension, i.e., Perfectionistic Concerns or Perfectionistic Strivings. Taking into account this criterion, in the case of other forms of perfectionism, effect sizes were analysed only for the perfectionism subscale of EDI. However, all measures were considered to calculate the total effect about the relationship between perfectionism and binge eating. Table 1 presents the average effect sizes for each meta-analysis together with their 95% confidence intervals and heterogeneity statistics. The global dimension of Perfectionistic Concerns and its respective indicators (Concern Over Mistakes, Socially Prescribed Perfectionism) exhibited a small-to-moderate positive relationship with binge eating (r+ = .27, .28, and .23, respectively), with the exception of Doubts About Actions that exhibited a moderate positive relationship (r+ = .34). All of these average correlation coefficients were statistically significant (p < .001). The global dimension of Perfectionistic Strivings and its indicator ‘Self-Oriented Perfectionism’ presented an average effect size under de cut-off value of .10 (r+ = .07 and .09, respectively), leading to a negligible relationship with binge eating. In addition, the indicator ‘Personal Standards’, although achieved a small positive relationship with binge eating, it did not reach statistical significance (r+ = .11; 95% CI: − .003–.22). Regarding the association between perfectionism assessed by using the EDI and binge eating, it did not reach a correlation > .10 according to Cohen’s criteria (r+ = .09). The heterogeneity test (Q) was statistically significant for all meta-analyses, except for the indicator ‘Doubts About Actions’ (p = .23). The heterogeneity indices (I2) ranged from 49.8 to 70.8%, suggesting the existence of moderate-to-large heterogeneity among the individual effect sizes.

Moderator analyses of the relationship between perfectionism and binge eating

Moderator analyses were conducted for the 33 independent global correlation coefficients obtained from each study. Moderator analyses were not carried out for the other more specific meta-analyses because none of them had 30 effect sizes at least. Categorical moderators analyzed by means of subgroup analysis (ANOVA) were the average age of the sample (adolescents 11–17, young adults 18–25, and adults > 25), sample type (clinical, non-clinical undergraduates, and non-clinical others), sample size (N = 80–200, 201–400, > 400), country (Australia, Belgium, Canada, USA, and Canada + UK), study design (cross-sectional vs. longitudinal), and instruments (perfectionisms scales, and binge eating scales). Continuous moderators analyzed by means of simple meta-regressions were the percentage of females, mean age of the sample and the publication year.

Table 2 presents the results of the ANOVAs conducted on the categorial variables. Five moderator variables showed a statistically significant association with the effect sizes: (a) the age, (b) the type of the sample used in the study, (c) the design, (d) the measurement instrument used to assess perfectionism, and (c) the measurement instrument used to assess binge eating. The age of the samples, categorized into adolescents, young adults, and adults, exhibited a statistically significant relationship with the effect sizes (p = .02; R2 = .20). Studies with samples composed by young adults showed a moderate-to-small association between perfectionism and binge eating (r+ = .22), in comparison with adolescents (r+ = .14) and adults (r+ = .03). In addition, stronger associations were observed when non-clinical samples (p = .001; R2 = .56), either undergraduates (r+ = .22) or others (r+ = .24), were employed, compared to clinical samples (r+ = .04). Regarding the study design (p = .004; R2 = .30), studies with longitudinal data did not reach a statistically significant perfectionism-binge eating association (r+ = .03), whereas cross-sectional studies reported a positive, significant relationship (r+ = .21). In the same line, the perfectionism scale exhibited a statistically significant association with the effect sizes (p = .03; R2 = .32). Specifically, the perfectionism scales than showed a positive, statistically significant average correlation with binge eating were FMPS versions (r+ = .26), HMPS versions (r+ = .21), and several measures combined (r+ = .15). Finally, the employed tool for assessing binge eating also exhibited a statistically significant association with the effect sizes (p < .001; R2 = .90). Moderate-to-small average correlations between perfectionism and binge eating were obtained for EDI versions (r+ = .30), frequency of binge eating (r+ = .30), MEBS (r+ = .30), measures combined (r+ = .28), EPSI (r+  = .28), BES (r+ = .26), EDDS (r+ = .23), and Binge Scale (r+ = .15).

Table 2 Results of the weighted ANOVAs for the influence of categorical variables on the effect sizes

Simple meta-regressions conducted on continuous moderators such as the percentage of females, mean age and publication year are presented in Table 3. None of these moderators exhibited a statistically significant association with the effect sizes.

Table 3 Results of the simple weighted meta-regressions of continuous moderators on the effect sizes

Analysis of publication bias

All the studies included in this meta-analysis were published papers (with one exception only), so statistical analyses were carried out to determine whether publication bias might be a threat to the validity of the results of the meta-analysis. Figure 4 presents a funnel plot relating the effect sizes with their standard errors. The Duval and Tweedie trim-and-fill method did not impute missing studies to symmetrize the funnel plot. In addition, the Egger’s test [95] applied to the intercept of a simple regression model of the effect sizes reached a non-statistically significant result [t(31) = 1.72, p = .09]. Therefore, publication bias can be discarded as a threat to the meta-analytic results.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Funnel plot of the association between Perfectionism and Binge Eating for the 33 global correlation coefficients (transformed into Fisher’s Z). Empty diamond represents the average Fisher’s Z for the 33 original effect sizes. Black diamond represents the average Fisher’s Z for the original effect sizes plus the imputed ones with the trim-and-fill method. As this method did not impute any additional effect size, the empty and black diamonds coincide

Discussion

The present meta-analysis of 30 studies, 33 samples, and 9392 participants represents the most comprehensive test of the perfectionism-binge eating relationship to date. The study found a positive and statistically significant correlation between overall perfectionism and binge eating, indicating that higher levels of perfectionism were associated with higher levels of binge eating. Specifically, a correlation coefficient of r+ = .17 was obtained, which, according to Cohen’s [19] guidelines, can be interpreted as reflecting a relationship of small magnitude. This positive and significant link was consistent across the two higher-order dimensions—Perfectionistic Concerns and Perfectionistic Strivings–, as well as its respective indicators (except for Personal Standards) and the EDI-Perfectionism subscale. However, variations regarding effect sizes associated to the correlation coefficients ranging from .07 to .34 were observed. Specific relationships are now discussed in detail.

Perfectionistic concerns and binge eating

Perfectionistic Concerns has been traditionally considered a clearly maladaptive component of perfectionism [100]. Thus, as expected, Perfectionistic Concerns showed a positive and significant association, of a moderate-to-small magnitude (r+ = .27), with binge eating. This link was consistent across the three indicators of Perfectionistic Concerns evaluated in this study—Concern Over Mistakes (r+ = .28), Socially Prescribed Perfectionism (r+ = .23), and Doubts About Actions (r+ = .34)—with small-to-moderate positive correlations. These findings provide additional empirical support for the original [85, 87] and reformulated [65] PMOBE according to which Socially Prescribed Perfectionism and Concern Over Mistakes promote vulnerability to binge eating by generating four binge eating maintenance variables (i.e., interpersonal discrepancies, low interpersonal esteem, depressive affect, and dietary restraint) that are viewed as both precursors and sequels of binge eating. However, neither the original nor the reformulated PMOBE considered Doubts About Actions as a possible explanatory factor of binge eating. In this sense, considering that Doubts About Actions-binge eating link was the only one that reached a moderate effect size, our results advise expanding the PMOBE including the possible role of Doubts About Actions, which could increase our understanding of binge eating beyond Socially Prescribed Perfectionism and Concern Over Mistakes. Doubts About Actions is defined as a nagging sense of uncertainty regarding the quality of one’s performance [39]. These annoying self-doubts might also confer vulnerability to binge eating through the four triggers stablished by the PMOBE. People high in Doubts About Actions appears to be more likely to experience interpersonal problems such as need for social approval, lack of sociability, interpersonal sensitivity, social anxiety, etc. [20, 23, 81]. That social disfunction vulnerability might predispose individuals with high levels of Doubts About Actions to perceive more interpersonal discrepancies and poor interpersonal esteem. Additionally, the frequent perception of being disappointing others might explain why doubting one’s performance is closely related with depressive symptoms [93], resulting, in turn, in self-defeating behaviors, such as binge eating, as a coping response.

On the other hand, both, the need for social approval and dietary restraint are implicated factors in the pathogenesis of binge eating [9, 56]. Under the influence of mass media, peers, and family, individuals high in Doubts About Actions, due to their frequent self-evaluation, may feel particularly pressured to be thin and engage in upward social comparison. That pursuit of the thin ideal, with subsequent body dissatisfaction, dieting restrictions, and unhealthy control eating behaviors predispose the onset of binge eating disorders [98]. That is, individuals characterized by high levels of Doubts About Actions may also unsuccessfully attempt to restrain their eating to gain social approval from significant others (e.g., family, peers, coaches, teachers…), compensating then that caloric deprivation by binge eating. Interestingly, Kim et al. [58] found that mothers and daughters’ Perfectionistic Concerns (conceptualized as the sum of Doubts About Actions and Concern Over Mistakes) play an important role in binge eating. Of the two indicators of mothers’ Perfectionistic Concerns, Doubts About Actions was the only one significantly correlated with daughter’s binge eating. In this sense, it may be that binge eating arises in daughters as a consequence of their attempts to diet to alter their shape to please their hypercritical mothers.

Perfectionistic strivings and binge eating

Contrary to Perfectionistic Concerns, the maladaptive or adaptive nature of Perfectionistic Strivings has been a matter of discussion (e.g., [33, 61, 75]. In this sense, our results evidenced that this higher-order dimension would be positively and significantly associated with binge eating (r+ = .07), although in a lesser magnitude than Perfectionistic Concerns. Of the two Perfectionistic Strivings indicators assessed in this study, only the Self-Oriented Perfectionism-binge eating link reached the statistical significance. However, it is worth note that the negligible average effect size obtained for the Self-Oriented Perfectionism-binge eating correlation (below .10) indicates that this association, although statistically significant, has no practical relevance. That statistical significance reached by both, Perfectionistic Strivings and Self-Oriented Perfectionism, might be explained by the large sample size; N = 3525 and 2380 participants, respectively, in comparison with the N = 966 participants for the Personal Standards calculations. Overall, these results suggest that the tendency to set extremely high standards and motivation to attain perfection has no relevant effect on this maladaptive eating behavior.

Other forms of perfectionism and binge eating

Other forms of perfectionism not classified as indicators of Perfectionistic Concerns and Perfectionistic Strivings were also considered in this meta-analysis. Unfortunately, the lack of studies prevented the analysis of all those other forms of perfectionism except for the perfectionism subscale of EDI. Surprisingly, EDI-Perfectionism relationship with binge eating reached a significant association but with values below .10. Again, it means that this relationship, although statistically significant, seems to be negligible for practical purposes. This is an interesting result, since the EDI-Perfectionism subscale was designed to measure perfectionism associated with eating disturbances. In this sense, Chang et al. [18] compared three measures of perfectionism (FMPS, HMPS and EDI-perfectionism) to identify the strongest unique perfectionism predictors of eating disturbances and health behaviors. Surprisingly, EDI-Perfectionism did not emerge as a significant unique predictor of any form of eating disturbances. Therefore, according to the authors, it calls into question studies that have employed that subscale as the only measure of perfectionism because their findings may have been subjected to the confusion of perfectionism (as a symptom of eating disturbances) with disturbed eating outcomes. Further, although the EDI-Perfectionism has been widely employed to assess perfectionism as a unidimensional construct, some research has evidenced that it is best represented by a multidimensional structure with items capturing a partial representation of Self-Oriented Perfectionism and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism (e.g., [86]. In this sense, we agree with Hewitt's et al. [50] idea about the multidimensionality of perfectionism and therefore it must be assessed using a multidimensional measure.

Moderator variables

Even though no evidence of publication bias was found as a threat to the validity of our results, a large heterogeneity among the individual effect sizes was reported. Five of the ten moderator variables analyzed in the meta-analysis had a statistical influence on the effect sizes for the perfectionism-binge eating correlations: the age, the type of the sample, the design, and the measurement instrument used to assess perfectionism and binge eating. Regarding the age of the sample, our results obtained strongest associations in young adult (18–25 years) samples than in adolescents and adults. This result makes sense considering that binge eating seems to be more common in young adulthood [107]. According to that, preventive interventions should be addressed particularly to adolescents and young adults.

Secondly, perfectionism was more strongly associated with binge eating when non-clinical samples were used in comparison with clinical samples. A possible explanation of this result would be that the association between perfectionism and binge eating was a consequence of an underlying third variable; fasting. It is important to highlight that clinical samples in this meta-analysis were comprised of individuals with Bulimia or other clinical conditions (Binge Eating Disorder, overweight or obesity) associated to binge eating symptomatology. Binge eating does not distinguish eating disorders, as it is a common symptom even in a subtype of Anorexia Nervosa [1]. However, under-eating and starvation predominate in anorectic patients in comparison with other clinical conditions—indeed, resulting in extremely low body weight. Thus, the perfectionism-binge eating link would be attributable to the presence of fasting among those who binge [34]. This fact also might explain why perfectionism has been considered as a specific risk factor for Bulimia and Anorexia Nervosa, but not for Binge Eating Disorder [28, 29], and why women with Anorexia Nervosa tend to be characterized by higher levels of perfectionism when compared with patients with Bulimia Nervosa [6, 37]. However, this result must be interpreted very cautiously as the negligible perfectionism-binge eating relationship found in clinical samples could be explained by the lack of variability in the scores reported by participants. Furthermore, an alternative explanation is insufficient statistical power, given that the sample size of studies with clinical samples is smaller in comparison with those with community samples. In the same line, considering that four of the seven samples with longitudinal data were composed by clinical population, it may also explain the weakest association between perfectionism and binge eating in studies with a longitudinal design than with a cross-sectional one.

Furthermore, results showed that the EDI-Perfectionism subscale was the least sensitive tool in identifying relationships between perfectionism and binge eating symptomatology. As mentioned above (see “Other forms of perfectionism and binge eating” Section), the EDI-Perfectionism consists of six items originally developed to assess perfectionism in the context of eating disturbances. Even though the subscale was created, and has been mostly employed, as a unidimensional measure of perfectionism, several studies advert that half of the items appear to reflect Self-Oriented Perfectionism whereas the other reflect Socially Prescribed Perfectionism (e.g., [86]. This tendency to jointly assess well-differentiated facets of perfectionism in a brief measure that partially represents more complex constructs could explain this unexpected result.

Finally, findings revealed that the perfectionism-binge eating association varied considerably depending on the tool employed to assess binge eating, being, in fact, the moderator variable that explained the highest proportion of variance (R2 = .90). Thus, even though most of the binge eating assessment tools provided small-to-moderate correlation coefficients, studies using EDE/EDE-Q versions reported non-significant correlations, and the study of Eddy et al. [24], using the BMS [96], reported a negative perfectionism-binge eating correlation. Unfortunately, the small number of studies per subgroup advise caution in interpreting these effect sizes. Therefore, to avoid biased or inaccuracy conclusions, we are going to focus on discussing mean effect sizes obtained from more than 3 studies (i.e., EDE/EDE-Q versions and EDI versions). Eleven studies analyzed the relationship between perfectionism and binge eating using different versions of the EDE or the EDE-Q. The EDE is an expert semi-structured interview that provides frequency data on key features of eating disorders. Although the EDE allows to assess both objective and subjective overeating, researchers usually resort to this interview to analyze objective binge eating operationalized as the number of episodes in the past 28 days [46, 59]. This tool is considered the gold standard for the diagnosis of Binge Eating Disorder [72]. Unfortunately, it has certain disadvantages for research purposes. For instance, it requires extensive interviewer training and familiarity with the tool to ensure competency as well as valid and reliable results [77]. These shortcomings were addressed by the EDE-Q, a self-report measure based on the EDE that assesses symptoms of eating disturbances over the past 28 days, providing investigators with a practical and inexpensive evaluation method. Using the EDE-Q it is possible to assess objective binge eating with two items that ask participants “how many times have you eaten what people would regard as an unusually large amount of food given the circumstances?” and “did you have a sense of having lost control over your eating at the time you were eating?”. However, these manner of analyzing objective binge eating through the EDE-Q has led to some controversy, suggesting that subjects, particularly those diagnosed with a Binge Eating Disorder, should be instructed in the construct of binge to avoid ambiguities in their interpretation [44].

The EDI-Bulimia is a 7-item self-report measure to assess the tendency toward episodes of uncontrollable overeating and the impulse to engage in self-induced vomiting. One problem of using this subscale for assessing binge eating is that, whereas there is an item clearly reflecting purge tendencies (“I have the thought of trying to vomit in other to lose weight”) and four items clearly addressing binge tendencies (“I stuff myself with food”, “I have gone on eating binges where I have felt that I could not stop”, “I think about bingeing”, and “I eat moderately in front of others and stuff myself when they are gone”), the remaining two items do not necessary reflect binge or purge tendencies (“I eat when I am upset” and “I eat or drink in secrecy”). Thus, this has led to disagreements between investigations when selecting the number of items (four to six) to assess binge eating.

One important question about our findings is why EDE/EDE-Q versions where the least sensitive instruments in identifying perfectionism-binge eating association (r+ = .02), whereas the EDI-Bulimia was one of the most sensitive tools to capture this relationship (r+ = .30). A tentative explanation may be that eight of the eleven studies using the EDE or EDE-Q versions were developed with clinical samples, whereas all studies using the EDI-Bulimia were based on community samples. Therefore, the lack of dispersion of data might justify weak correlation coefficients between perfectionism and binge eating. An alternative explanation involves the fact that EDE/EDE-Q provide a clear differentiation between objective and subjective overeating, whereas EDI-Bulimia does not distinguish both constructs. Thus, whereas some items of EDI-Bulimia are referred to objective binge episodes (i.e., “I have gone on eating binges where I have felt that I could not stop”), others are assessing feelings or thoughts that might emerge even in the absence of consuming a large amount of food (i.e., “I think about bingeing”).

Limitations and suggestions for future research

It is important to highlight that this study has several limitations. First, it is possible that this meta-analysis has captured more comprehensively Perfectionistic Concerns (comprised of four indicators: Concern Over Mistakes, Socially Prescribed Perfectionism, Doubts About Actions, and Perfectionistic Discrepancies) than Perfectionistic Strivings (compromised by only two indicators: Personal Standards and Self-Oriented Perfectionism). Secondly, most research on the perfectionism-binge eating link is on trait perfectionism. Thus, future studies should address this gap analyzing possible implications of other forms of perfectionism, such as perfectionistic self-presentation [51] or automatic thoughts [32] on binge eating symptomatology. Moreover, most of the studies included were performed in USA or Canada, which might limit the generalization of our results. Because perfectionism’ outcomes might be influenced by sociocultural factors [66], an important avenue for future research would be to analyze how perfectionism is related with binge eating across cultures and ethnic minorities. Similarly, more longitudinal studies, not only with clinical participants but also with community samples are also required to test if perfectionism confers risk for binge eating over time. Also, future investigators should consider adopting a multidimensional approach as a more comprehensive assessment of perfectionism rather than using unidimensional measures such as the EDI-Perfectionism. Finally, considering the huge amount of variance explained by the binge eating tool employed, researchers must be particularly thoughtful in selecting the measurement instrument. In this sense, although the distinction between subjective and objective binges seems not to be clinically useful [74], we suggest that future research investigates if the perfectionism-binge eating link would be the same regardless of subjective or objective overeating is considered (Additional file 1).

Conclusions

This meta-analysis sheds light on the perfectionism-binge eating link, extending current knowledge about the association of this trait of personality and psychopathology [60]. Decades of research suggest that perfectionism is a central core of eating disturbances, and our study corroborates that binge eating is not an exception [22, 30, 57]. In fact, all the perfectionism indicators analyzed in this study were positively and statistically associated with binge eating, except for Personal Standards whose association did not reach the statistical significance. Our meta-analysis also evidenced that Perfectionistic Concerns are linked to more severe binge eating symptomatology than Perfectionistic Strivings, whose association with binge eating appears to be negligible. Accordingly, our findings underscore the importance of addressing perfectionism, particularly Perfectionistic Concerns, as a potential risk factor for binge eating symptomatology.

Availability of data and materials

The data that supports the findings of this study are available in the supplementary material of this article.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). 2013. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

  2. American Psychological Association. (n.d.). Binge eating. In APA dictionary of psychology. Retrieved 06 June 2022. from https://dictionary.apa.org/binge-eating

  3. Bagby RM, Parker JDA, Joffe RT, Buis T. Reconstruction and validation of the depressive experiences questionnaire. Assessment. 1994;1:59–68. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191194001001009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Bardone-Cone AM, Abramson LY, Vohs KD, Heatherton TF, Joiner TE. Predicting bulimic symptoms: an interactive model of self-efficacy, perfectionism, and perceived weight status. Behav Res Ther. 2006;44:27–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2004.09.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Bardone-Cone AM, Joiner TE, Crosby RD, Crow SJ, Klein MH, Grange DL, Mitchell JE, Peterson CB, Wonderlich SA. Examining a psychosocial interactive model of binge eating and vomiting in women with bulimia nervosa and subthreshold bulimia nervosa. Behav Res Therapy. 2008;46:887–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2008.04.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Bardone-Cone AM, Wonderlich SA, Frost RO, Bulik CM, Mitchell JE, Uppala S, Simonich H. Perfectionism and eating disorders: current status and future directions. Clin Psychol Rev. 2007;27:384–405.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Bardone-Cone AM, Brownstone LM, Higgins MK, Harney MB, Fitzsimmons-Craft EE. Predicting difficulties controlling overeating and drinking when experiencing negative affect in undergraduate women. J Soc Clin Psychol 2012;31(10):1051–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Blatt SJ, D’Afflitti JP, Quinlan DM. Experiences of depression in normal young adults. J Abnorm Psychol. 1976;95:383–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Blomquist KK, Ansell EB, White MA, Masheb RM, Grilo CM. Interpersonal problems and developmental trajectories of binge eating disorder. Compr Psychiatry. 2012;53:1088–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2012.05.003.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Boone L. Are attachment styles differentially related to interpersonal perfectionism and binge eating symptoms? Pers Individ Differ. 2013;54:931–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.01.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Boone L, Soenens B, Vansteenkiste M, Braet C. Is there a perfectionist in each of us? An experimental study on perfectionism and eating disorder symptoms. Appetite. 2012;59:531–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.06.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Boone L, Vansteenkiste M, Soenens B, van der Kaap- Deeder J. Self-critical perfectionism and binge eating symptoms: a longitudinal test of the intervening role of psychological need frustration. J Couns Psychol. 2014;61(3):363–73. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036418.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Borenstein MJ, Hedges LV, Higgins JP, Rothstein HR. Introduction to meta-analysis (2nd edn.). Wiley; 2019.

  14. Borenstein MJ, Hedges LV, Higgins J, Rothstein HR. Comprehensive meta-analysis (version 3.3). Biostat Inc.; 2014.

  15. Brosof LC, Levinson CA. Social appearance anxiety and dietary restraint as mediators between perfectionism and binge eating: a six month three wave longitudinal study. Appetite. 2016;1(108):335–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2016.10.015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Burton AL, Abbott MJ. Conceptualising binge eating: a review of the theoretical and empirical literature. Behav Chang. 2017;34(3):168–98. https://doi.org/10.1017/bec.2017.12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Card NA. Applied meta-analysis for social Science research. Guilford Press; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Chang EC, Ivezaj C, Downey CA, Kashima Y, Morady AR. Complexities of measuring perfectionism: three popular perfectionism measures and their relations with eating disturbances and health behaviors in a female college student sample. Eat Behav. 2008;9:102–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2007.06.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol Bull. 1992;112:155–9. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Cox SL, Chen JW. Perfectionism: a contributor to social anxiety and its cognitive processes. Aust J Psychol. 2015;67(4):231–40. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajpy.12079.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Cox BJ, Enns MW, Clara LP. The multidimensional structure of perfectionism in clinically distressed and college student samples. Psychol Assess. 2002;14:365–73. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.14.3.365.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Dahlenburg SC, Gleaves DH, Hutchinson AD. Anorexia nervosa and perfectionism: a meta-analysis. Int J Eat Disord. 2019;52(3):219–29. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.23009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Dimaggio G, Lysaker PH, Calarco T, Pedone R, Marsigli N, Riccardi I, Sabatelli B, Carcione A, Paviglianiti A. Perfectionism and personality disorders as predictors of symptoms and interpersonal problems. Am J Psychother. 2015;69(3):317–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Eddy KT, Tanofsky-Kraff MT, Thompson-Brenner H, Herzog DB, Brown TA, Ludwig DS. Eating disorder pathology among overweight treatment-seeking youth: clinical correlates and cross-sectional risk modeling. Behav Res Ther. 2007;45:2360–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2007.03.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Fairburn CG, Beglin SJ. Assessment of eating disorders: interview or self-report questionnaire? Int J Eat Disord. 1994;16:363–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Fairburn CG, Beglin SJ. Eating disorder examination questionnaire (EDE-Q 6.0). In: Fairburn CG, editor. Cognitive behavior therapy and eating disorders. Appendix B, 309–314. Guilford Press; 2008.

  27. Fairburn C, Cooper P. The eating disorders examination. In: Fairburn C, Wilson G, editors. Binge eating: nature, assessment and treatment. Guilford; 1993. p. 317–360

  28. Fairburn CG, Cooper Z, Doll HA, Welch SL. Risk factors for anorexia nervosa: three integrated case-control comparisons. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56:468–76. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.56.5.468.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Fairburn CG, Doll HA, Welch SL, Hay PJ, Davies BA, O’Connor ME. Risk factors for binge eating disorder: a community-based, case-control study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55:425–32. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.55.5.425.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Farstad SM, McGeown LM, von Ranson KM. Eating disorders and personality, 2004–2016: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.04.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Flett GL, Hewitt PL. The multidimensional discrepancy inventory (In preparation).

  32. Flett GL, Hewitt PL, Demerjian A, Sturman ED, Sherry SB, Cheng W. Perfectionistic automatic thoughts and psychological distress in adolescents: an analysis of the perfectionism cognitions inventory. J Ratl Emot Cogn Behav Ther. 2012;30(2):91–104. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10942-011-0131-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Flett GL, Hewitt PL. Positive versus negative perfectionism in psychopathology: a comment on Slade and Owens’s dual process mod-el. Behav Modif. 2006;30:472–95. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445506288026.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Forbush K, Heatherton TF, Keel PK. Relationship between perfectionism and specific disordered eating behaviors. Int J Eat Disord. 2007;40:37–41. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Forbush KT, Wildes JE, Pollack LO, Dunbar D, Luo J, Patterson K, Stone A. Development and validation of the eating pathology symptoms inventory (EPSI). Psychol Assess. 2013;25(3):859–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Forney KJ, Schwendler T, Ward RM. Examining similarities in eating pathology, negative affect, and perfectionism among peers: a social network analysis. Appetite. 2019;137:236–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2019.03.013.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Franco-Paredes K, Mancilla-Diaz JM, Vázquez-Arevalo R, López-Aguilar X, Álvarez-Rayon G. Perfectionism and eating disorders: a review of the literature. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2005;13:61–70. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Frank GKW, Berner LA. Binge eating. A transdiagnostic psychopathology. Springer; 2020.

  39. Frost RO, Marten P, Lahart C, Rosenblate R. The dimensions of perfectionism. Cogn Ther Res. 1990;14:449–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Garner DM. Eating disorder inventory-2 professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources. 1991.

  41. Garner DM, Olmstead MP, Polivy J. Development and validation of a multidimensional eating disorder inventory for anorexia nervosa and bulimia. Int J Eat Disord. 1983;2:15–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Gilbert P, Clarke M, Hempel S, Miles JNV, Irons C. Criticizing and reassuring oneself: an exploration of forms, styles and reasons in female students. Br J Clin Psychol. 2004;43(Pt 1):31–50. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466504772812959.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Glynn SM, Ruderman AJ. The development and validation of an eating self-efficacy scale. Cogn Ther Res. 1986;10:403–20. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01173294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Goldfein JA, Devlin MJ, Kamenetz C. Eating disorder examination-questionnaire with and without instruction to assess binge eating in patients with binge eating disorder. Int J Eat Disord. 2005;37(2):107–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Gormally J, Black S, Daston S, Rasdin D. The assessment of binge eating severity among obese persons. Addict Behav 1982;7(1):47–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4603(82)90024-7

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Groleau P, Steiber H, Bruce K, Israel M, Sycz L, Ouellette AS, Badawi G. Childhood emotional abuse and eating symptoms in bulimic disorders: an examination of possible mediating variables. Int J Eat Disord. 2012;45:326–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Hawkins RC, Clement PF. Development and construct-validation of a self-report measure of binge eating tendencies. Addict Behav. 1980;5:219–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Heard-Egbert A, Smith K, Goldschmidt AB. Psychosocial correlates of binge eating. In: Frank GKW, Berner LA editors. Binge eating. A transdiagnostic psychopathology. Springer; 2020. p. 41–58

  49. Hewitt PL, Flett GL. Perfectionism in the self and social contexts: Conceptualization, assessment, and association with psychopathology. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1991;60:456–70. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.60.3.456.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Hewitt PL, Flett GL, Besser A, Sherry SB, McGee B. Perfectionism is multidimensional: a reply to Shafran, Cooper and Fairburn (2002). Behav Res Ther. 2003a;41(10):1221–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00021-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Hewitt PL, Flett GL, Sherry SB, Habke M, Parkin M, Lam RW, McMurtry B, Ediger E, Fairlie P, Stein MB. The interpersonal expression of perfection: perfectionistic self-presentation and psychological distress. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003b;84(6):1303–25. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.6.1303.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Hewitt PL, Habke AM, Lee-Baggley DL, Sherry SB, Flett GL. The impact of perfectionistic self-presentation on the cognitive, affective, and physiological experience of a clinical interview. Psychiatry. 2008;71(2):93–122. https://doi.org/10.1521/psyc.2008.71.2.93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Hilbert A, Hartmann AS, Czaja J. Child eating disorder examination-questionnaire: Evaluation der deutschsprachigen Version des Esssto¨rungsfragebogens fu¨r Kinder [Evaluation of the german-speaking version of the child eating disorder examination questionnaire]. Klin Diag und Eval 2008;1:447–464

    Google Scholar 

  54. Hooley JM, Ho DT, Slater JA, Lockshin A. Pain insensitivity and self-harming behavior. In: Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Research in Psychopathology. 2002.

  55. Jones M, Crowther J. Predicting the onset of inappropriate compensatory behaviors in undergraduate college women. Eat Behav. 2013;14:17–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2012.10.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Kalarchian MA, Marcus MD. Eating disorders: binge eating. In: Caballero B editor. Encyclopedia of human nutrition. Academic Press; 2023. p. 120–125

  57. Kehayes IL, Smith MM, Sherry SB, Vidovic V, Saklofske DH. Are perfectionism dimensions risk factors for bulimic symptoms? A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Pers Individ Differ. 2019;138:117–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.09.022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Kim AJ, Sherry SB, Stewart SH, Smith MM, Popowich AD, Mushquash AR. Perfectionistic concerns and binge eating in mother-daughter dyads: an actor-partner interdependence model. Eat Behav. 2023;48:101702. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2023.101702.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Lampard AM, Byrne SM. An evaluation of the enhanced cognitive-behavioural model of bulimia nervosa. Behav Res Pract. 2011;49:529–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2011.06.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Limburg K, Watson HJ, Hagger MS, Egan SJ. The relationship between perfectionism and psychopathology: a meta-analysis. J Clin Psychol. 2017;73(10):1301–26. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22435.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Lo A, Abbott MJ. Review of the theoretical, empirical, and clinical status of adaptive and maladaptive perfectionism. Behav Change 2013;30:96–116. https://doi.org/10.1017/bec.2013.9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. López-López JA, Marín-Martínez F, Sánchez-Meca J, van den Noortgate W, Viechtbauer W. Estimation of the predictive power of the model in mixed-effects meta-regression: a simulation study. Br J Math Stat Psychol. 2014;67(1):30–48. https://doi.org/10.1111/bmsp.12002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Lowe MR, Gleaves DH, Murphy-Eberenz KP. On the relation of dieting and bingeing in bulimia nervosa. J Abnorm Psychol. 1998;107:263–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Luo J, Forbush KT, Williamson JA, Markon KE, Pollack LO. How specific are the relationships between eating disorder behaviors and perfectionism? Eat Behav. 2013;14:291–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.04.003.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Mackinnon SP, Sherry SB, Graham AR, Stewart SH, Sherry DL, Allen SL, Fitzpatrick S, McGrath DS. Reformulating and testing the perfectionism model of binge eating among undergraduate women: a short-term, three-wave longitudinal study. J Couns Psychol. 2011;58(4):630–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Marten-Dibartolo P, Rendón MJ. A critical examination of the construct of perfectionism and its relationship to mental health in Asian and African Americans using a cross-cultural framework. Clin Psychol Rev. 2012;32(3):139–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.09.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Merwin KE, Mackinnon SP, O’Connor RM, Flett GF. Socially prescribed perfectionism predicts next-day binge eating behaviors over 20-days. J Couns Psychol. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000600.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Migliore DA. Predictors of binge eating. Perfectionism, distress tolerance and eating self-efficacy. [Doctoral dissertation, State University of New Jersey]. Rutgers University Community Repository; 2010.

  69. Minnich AM, Gordon KH, Holm-Denoma JM, Troop-Gordon W. A test of an interactive model of binge eating among undergraduate men. Eat Behav. 2014;15:625–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014.08.016.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Mushquash AR, Sherry SB. Understanding the socially prescribed perfectionist’s cycle of self-defeat: a 7-day, 14-occasion daily diary study. J Res Pers. 2012;46:700–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2012.08.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Mushquash AR, Sherry SB. Testing the perfectionism model of binge eating in mother–daughter dyads: a mixed longitudinal and daily diary study. Eat Behav. 2013;14:171–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.02.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. National Institute for Health and Care Excellence. Eating disorders: recognition and treatment (full guideline). NICE. 2017. https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng69/evidence/full-guideline-pdf-161214767896.

  73. Neyland HMK, Shank LM, Lavender JM. Theoretical development and maintenance models of binge eating. In: Frank GKW, Berner LA, editors. Binge eating. A transdiagnostic psychopathology. Springer; 2020. pp. 69–82

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  74. Niego SH, Pratt EM, Agras WS. Subjective or objective binge: is the distinction valid? Int J Eating Disord. 1997;22(3):291–8. https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1098-108x(199711)22:3%3c291::aid-eat8%3e3.0.co;2-i.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Owens RG, Slade PD. So perfect it’s positively harmful? Re-flections on the adaptiveness and maladaptiveness of positive and negative perfectionism. Behav Modif. 2008;43:472–95. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445508319667.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, Shamseer L, Tetzlaff JM, Akl EA, Brennan SE, Chou R, Glanville J, Grimshaw JM, Hróbjartsson A, Lalu MM, Li T, Loder EW, Mayo-Wilson E, McDonald S, McGuinness LA, Stewart LA, Thomas J, Tricco AC, Welch VA, Whiting P, Moher D. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021;71:237. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.n71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Peyser D, Cambell M, Sysko R. Epidemiology of binge eating. In: Frank GKW, Berner LA editors. Binge eating. A transdiagnostic psychopathology. Springer; 2020. p. 3–12

  78. Pratt EM, Telch CF, Labouvie EW, Wilson GT, Stewart-Agras W. Perfectionism in women with binge eating disorder. Int J Eat Disord. 2001;29(2):177–86. https://doi.org/10.1002/1098-108x(200103)29:2%3c177::aid-eat1007%3e3.0.co;2-x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Ranson KM, Klump KL, Lacono WG, McGue M. The minnesota eating behavior survey: a brief measure of disordered eating attitude and behaviors. Eat Behav 2005;6(4):373–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2004.12.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Rosenberger PH, Henderson KE, Grilo CM. Correlates of body image dissatisfaction in extremely obese female bariatric surgery candidates. Obes Surg. 2006;16:1331–6. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023505108426.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Rosser S, Issakidis C, Peters L. Perfectionism and social phobia: relationship between the constructs and impact on cognitive behavior therapy. Cogn Ther Res. 2003;27(2):143–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Rothstein HR, Sutton AJ, Borenstein M, editors. Publication bias in meta-analysis: Prevention, assessment, and adjustments. Wiley; 2005.

  83. Sánchez-Meca J, Marín-Martínez F. Confidence intervals for the overall effect size in random-effects meta-analysis. Psychol Methods. 2008;13:31–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Sehm M, Warschburger P. Prospective associations between binge eating and psychological risk factors in adolescence. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2016;47(5):770–84. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2016.1178124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Sherry SB, Hall PA. The perfectionism model of binge eating: tests of an integrative model. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2009;96(3):690–709. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014528.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Sherry SB, Hewitt PL, Besser A, McGee BJ, Flett GL. Self-oriented and socially prescribed perfectionism in the eating disorder inventory perfectionism subscale. Int J Eating Disord. 2003;35:69e79.

    Google Scholar 

  87. Sherry SB, Sabourin BC, Hall PA, Hewitt PL, Flett GL, Gralnick TM. model of binge eating: testing unique contributions, mediating mechanisms, and cross-cultural similarities using a daily diary methodology. Psychol Addict Behav. 2014;28(4):1230–9. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037939.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Sherry SB, Stoeber J, Ramasubbu C. Perfectionism explains variance in self-defeating behaviors beyond self-criticism: evidence from a cross-national sample. Pers Individ Differ. 2016;95:196–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.02.059.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Short MM, Mushquash AR, Sherry SB. Perseveration moderates the relationship between perfectionism and binge eating: a multi-method daily diary study. Eat Behav. 2013;14:394–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.06.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Slane JD, Burt SA, Klump KL. The road less traveled: associations between externalizing behaviors and eating pathology. Int J Eat Disord. 2010;43:149–60. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.20680.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Slaney RB, Rice KG, Mobley M, Trippi J, Ashby JS. The revised almost perfect scale. Meas Eval Couns Dev. 2001;34:130–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Smith MM, Sherry SB, Gautreau CM, Stewart SH, Saklofske DH, Mushquash AR. Are perfectionistic concerns an antecedent of or a consequence of binge eating, or both? A short-term four wave longitudinal study of undergraduate women. Eat Behav. 2017;26:23–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2017.01.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Smith MM, Sherry SB, Rnic K, Saklofske DH, Enns M, Gralick T. Perfectionism dimensions vulnerability factors for depressive symptoms after controlling for neuroticism? A meta-analysis of 10 longitudinal studies. Eur J Pers. 2016;30(2):201–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  94. Stairs AM, Smith GT, Zapolski TC, Combs JL, Settles RE. Clarifying the construct of perfectionism. Assessment. 2012;19(2):146–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Sterne JAC, Egger M. Regression methods to detect publication and other bias in meta-analysis. In: Rothstein HR, Sutton AJ, Borenstein M, editors. Publication bias in metaanalysis: prevention, assessment and adjustments. Wiley; 2005. p. 99–100.

    Google Scholar 

  96. Stice E. Modeling of eating pathology and social reinforcement of the thin-ideal predict onset of bulimic symptoms. Behav Res Ther. 1998;36:931–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Stice E, Telch CE, Rizvi SL. Development and validation of the eating disorder diagnostic scale: a brief self-report measure of anorexia, bulimia, and binge-eating disorder. Psychol Assess. 2000;12:123–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.l2.2.123.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Stice E, Van Ryzin MJ. A prospective test of the temporal sequencing of risk factor emergence in the dual pathway model of eating disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 2019;128(2):119–28. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000400.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Stoeber J. The psychology of perfectionism: an introduction. In: Stoeber J, editor. The psychology of perfectionism. Routledge; 2018. p. 3–16.

    Google Scholar 

  100. Stoeber J, Otto K. Positive conceptions of perfectionism: approaches, evidence, challenges. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2006;10:295–319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Striegel-Moore RH, Dohm FA, Solomon EE, Fairburn CG, Pike KM, Wilfley DE. Subthreshold binge eating disorder. Int J Eating Disorders. 2000;227(3):270–8. https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1098-108x(200004)27:3<270::aid-eat3>3.0.co;2-1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  102. Thurfjell B, Edlund B, Arinell H, Hägglöf B, Garner DM, Engström I. Eating disorder inventory for children (EDI-C): effects of age and gender in a Swedish sample. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2004;12:256–64. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.583.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  103. Viechtbauer W, López-López JA, Sánchez-Meca J, Marín-Martínez F. A comparison of procedures to test for moderators in mixed-effects meta-regression models. Psychol Methods. 2015;20:360–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. Waltmann M, Herzog N, Horstmann A, Deserno L. Loss of control overeating: a systematic review of task-based research into impulsive and compulsive processes in binge eating. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;129:330–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Watson HJ, Steele AL, Bergin JL, Fursland A, Wade TD. Bulimic symptomatology: the role of adaptive perfectionism, shape and weight concern, and self-esteem. Behav Res Ther. 2011;49:565–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2011.04.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Welch E, Miller JL, Ghaderi A, Vaillancourt T. Does perfectionism mediate or moderate the relation between body dissatisfaction and disordered eating attitudes and behaviors? Eat Behav. 2009;10:168–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2009.05.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Wick MR, Fitzgerald EH, Keel PK. Epidemiology of binge eating. In: Frank GKW, Berner LA editors. Binge eating. A transdiagnostic psychopathology. Springer; 2020. p. 3–12

  108. Zelkowitz RL, Cole DA. Longitudinal relations of self-criticism with disordered eating behaviors and nonsuicidal self-injury. Int J Eat Disord. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.23284.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the Department of Behavioral Sciences and Health (Miguel Hernández University of Elche) for its support for Dr. María Vicent to conduct a postdoctoral research stay under the supervision of Dr. María José Quiles from January 1st, 2019 to August 1st, 2021, when Dr. María Vicent enjoyed a research-leave from the University of Alicante, where she is located. This article is the result of the research work initiated during that period.

Funding

This research was funded by the Valencian Government (Spain) with the grant number CIGE/2021/021 awarded to MV.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

MV: conceptualization, data curation, resources, investigation, writing—original draft, Writing—Review & Editing, project administration. CG: software, data curation, validation, resources, formal analysis, Writing—Review & Editing, project administration. MJQ: conceptualization, investigation, Writing—Review & Editing. JSM: methodology, software, recourse, formal analysis, Writing—Review & Editing, supervision. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to María Vicent.

Ethics declarations

Ethical approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Appendix 1 Characteristics of studies included in the meta-analysis

Appendix 1 Characteristics of studies included in the meta-analysis

Study

Sample

Measures

N

Sample type

Female %

M age

SD age

Nationality

Design

Perfectionism

Binge Eating

Bardone-Cone et al. [4]

406

Community: undergraduates

100

18.7

0.97

USA

Longitudinal

EDI: Perfectionism

EDE-Q: N of weeks with BE

Bardone-Cone et al. [5] Sample 1

180

Clinical: bulimic with binge eating

100

25.67

8.85

USA

Longitudinal

FMPS: Concern Over Mistakes

FMPS: Personal Standards

EDE-Q: Frequency of BE

Bardone-Cone et al. [5] Sample 2

169

Clinical: bulimic with vomiting

100

25.67

8.85

USA

Longitudinal

FMPS: Concern Over Mistakes

FMPS: Personal Standards

EDE-Q- Frequency of BE

Bardone-Cone et al. [7]

426

Community: undergraduates

100

18.6

0.97

USA

Longitudinal

HMPS: Socially Prescribed Perfectionism

HMPS: Self-Oriented Perfectionism

ESS-Negative Affect (an item subset)

Boone et al. [11]

100

Community: undergraduates

100

20.6

2.24

Belgium

Cross-sectional

FMPS: Concern Over Mistakes + Doubts About Actions

FMPS: Personal Standards

EDI-2: Bulimia (4 items about BE)

Boone [10]

326

Community: high school students

57

17.1

1.13

Belgium

Cross-sectional

PSPS: Perfectionistic self-promotion

HMPS: Socially Prescribed Perfectionism

EDI-2: Bulimia (6 items about BE)

Boone et al. [12]

566

Community: high school students

71

13.28

0.89

Belgium

Cross-sectional and Longitudinal

FMPS: Concern Over Mistakes + Doubts About Actions

EDI-2: Bulimia (6 items about BE)

Brosof and Levinson [15]

300

Community: undergraduates

100

18

1.05

USA

Cross-sectional and Longitudinal

FMPS: Concern Over Mistakes

EDI-2: Bulimia (5 items about BE)

Eddy et al. [24]

122

Clinical: overweight or risk

55,74

11.49

2.3

USA

Cross-sectional

EDI-2: Perfectionism

BMS: BE

Forney et al. [36]

89

Community: undergraduates

93.26

20.38

1.31

USA

Cross-sectional

M-CUP: Reactivity to Mistakes

EPSI: BE

Groleau et al. [46]

176

Clinical: bulimia

100

24.95

5.52

Canada

Cross-sectional

EDI-2: Perfectionism

EDE: Frequency of BE

Jones and Crowther [55]

237

Community: undergraduates

100

19.4

3.9

USA

Cross-sectional

EDI: Perfectionism

BE = 1 No BE = 0

Lampard et al. [59]

162

Clinical: bulimia

100

26.2

7.6

Australia

Cross-sectional

EDI-2: Self-Oriented Perfectionism

EDE: Frequency of BE

Lou et al. [64]

407

Community: adults

47

38.24

13.51

USA

Cross-sectional

FMPS: Concern Over Mistakes

FMPS: Personal Standards

FMPS: Parental Criticism

Frequency of BE

Mackinnon et al. [65]

200

Community: undergraduates

100

19.86

3.02

USA

Cross-sectional and Longitudinal

SF-FMPS: Concern Over Mistakes

SF-FMPS: Personal Standards

EDDS: BE (7 items)

Merwin et al. [67]

263

Community: emerging adults

79,80

21.37

1.89

Canada

Longitudinal

SF-HMPS: Self-Oriented Perfectionism

SF-HMPS: Socially Prescribed Perfectionism

SF-HMPS: Other Oriented Perfectionism

EDDS: BE (7 items)

Migliore [68]

126

Clinical: binge eaters

100

USA

Cross-sectional

HMPS: Self-Oriented Perfectionism

HMPS: Socially Prescribed Perfectionism

EDE-Q 6.0: BE

Minnich et al. [69]

302

Community: undergraduates

0

19.2

1.3

USA

Cross-sectional and Longitudinal

EDI: Perfectionism

BES

Mushquash y Sherry [70]

317

Community: undergraduates

77,92

20.32

4.34

Canada

Cross-sectional

EDI: Perfectionism

APS-R: Perfectionistic Discrepancies

DEQ-R: Perfectionistic Discrepancies

MDI: Perfectionistic Discrepancies

FMPS: Parental Expectation

FMPS: Parental Criticism PSPS

HMPS: Socially Prescribed Perfectionism

HMPS: Self-Oriented Perfectionism

EDI: Bulimia (4 items about BE)

Mushquash y Sherry [71]

218

Community: undergraduates

100

19.99

3.15

Canada

Cross-sectional and Longitudinal

SF-HMPS: Socially Prescribed Perfectionism (items referred to mothers)

EDI: Bulimia (an item subset focused on BE)

Pratt et al. [78]

219

Clinical: obese with binge eating disorder

100

USA

Cross-sectional

HMPS: Self-Oriented Perfectionism

HMPS: Socially Prescribed Perfectionism

HMPS: Other Oriented Perfectionism

BES

Rosenberger et al. [80]

131

Clinical: severely obese

100

41.8

10.9

USA

Cross-sectional

EDI: Perfectionism

EDE-Q: Frequency of BE

Sehm y Warschburger [84]

516

Community: adolescents

0

14.37

1.56

Germany

Cross-sectional and Longitudinal

EDI-C: Perfectionism

ChEDE-Q: N of days with BE

Sherry et al. [88]

524

Community: undergraduates

81,11

20.09

3.43

Canada and UK

Cross-sectional

FMPS: Concern Over Mistakes

FMPS: Doubts About Actions

DEQ: Self-Criticism

HMPS: Socially Prescribed Perfectionism

EDI: Bulimia (4 items about BE)

Sherry and Hall [85]

566

Community: undergraduates

100

Canada

Cross-sectional

HMPS: Self-Oriented Perfectionist

HMPS + FMPS + EDI: Socially Prescribed Perfectionism

MDI: Interpersonal Discrepancies

BULIT-R + EDI + EDDS: a subset of BE items

Short et al. [89]

317

Community: undergraduates

77,92

20.32

4.34

Canada

Cross-sectional

FMPS: Doubts About Actions

EDDS: BE

Slane et al. [90] Sample 1

344

Community: twins

100

20.89

2.33

USA

Cross-sectional

FMPS: Concern Over Mistakes

MEBS: BE

Slane et al. [90] Sample 2

216

Community: twins

0

20.89

2.33

USA

Cross-sectional

FMPS: Concern Over Mistakes

MEBS: BE

Smith et al. [92]

200

Community: undergraduates

100

19.9

3.02

Canada

Cross-sectional and Longitudinal

SF-FMPS: Concern Over Mistakes

SF-FMPS: Doubts About Actions

SF-HMPS: Socially Prescribed Perfectionism

BULIT-R + EDI + EDDS: a subset of BE items

Watson et al. [105] Sample 1

238

Clinical: eating disorder with binge eating

100

26.03

8.5

Canada

Longitudinal

EDI: Self-Oriented Perfectionism + FMPS: Personal Standards

EDE-Q: Frequency of BE

Watson et al. [105] Sample 2

258

Clinical: eating disorder with purging

100

26.03

8.5

Canada

Longitudinal

EDI: Self-Oriented Perfectionism + FMPS: Personal Standards

EDE-Q: Frequency of BE

Welch et al. [106]

520

Community: undergraduates

100

20.89

4.43

Canada

Cross-sectional

EDI-2: Perfectionism

HMPS: Self-Oriented Perfectionism

HMPS: Socially Prescribed Perfectionism

HMPS: Other Oriented Perfectionism

Binge Scale

Zelkowitz and Cole [108]

251

Community: undergraduates

79,50

19.1

1.23

USA

Cross-sectional

DEQ: Self-Criticism + SRS + FSC: Self-Criticism

EDE-Q: Frequency of BE

  1. EDI, Eating Disorder Inventory [41], EDE-Q, Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire [25], ESS, Eating Self-Efficacy Scale [43], EDI-2, Eating Disorder Inventory [40], BMS, Bulimic Modeling Scale [96], M-CUP, Measure of Constructs Underlying Perfectionism [94], EPSI, Eating Pathology Symptom Inventory [35], EDE, Eating Disorders Examination [27], EDDS, Eating Disorder Diagnostic Scale [97], EDE-Q 6.0, Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire 6.0 [26], BES, Binge Eating Scale [45], ChEDE-Q, Child Eating Disorder Examination-Questionnaire [53], BULIT-R, Bulimia Test-Revised [63], MEBS, Minnesota Eating Behavior Survey [79], Binge Scale [47], FSC, Self-Criticism/Self-Reassurance Scale [42], SRS, Self-Rating Scale [54], FMPS, Frost’s Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale [39], SF-FMPS, The Short Form of the Frost’s Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale [21], PSPS, Perfectionistic Self-Presentation Scale [51], HMPS, Hewitt's Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale [49], SF-HMPS, The Short Form of the Hewitt’s Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale [52], EDI-C, Eating Disorder Inventory-Children [102], DEQ, Depressive Experiencies Questionnaire [8], DEQ-R, Reconstructed Depressive Experiences Questionnaire [3], APS-R, Almost Perfect Scale-Revised [91], MDI, Multidimensional Discrepancy Inventory (MDI, Flett and Hewitt [31], BE, Binge Eating.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Vicent, M., Gonzálvez, C., Quiles, M.J. et al. Perfectionism and binge eating association: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Eat Disord 11, 101 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-023-00817-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-023-00817-9

Keywords