Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Covert therapeutic micro-processes in non-recovered eating disorders with childhood trauma: an interpersonal process recall study

Abstract

Method

To uncover therapeutic micro-processes from the perspectives of eating disorder (ED) treatment non-responders with childhood trauma (CT) late effects, we explored in-session experiences of poor long-term outcome patients. Female inpatients aged 28–59 (M = 40.2, SD = 5.0) from a randomised trial comparing Compassion Focused Therapy for EDs (n = 3) with Cognitive Behavioural Therapy for EDs (n = 3) were interviewed with video-assisted recall about a self-selected session. Data were analysed through Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis (IPA) with Grounded Theory (GT) elements.

Results

Covert patient strategies included self-effacement, regulating therapeutic distance to open up, and engaging with reflective rather than experiential interventions. First, self-effacement included submissive, passive or pretend responses to perceived criticising or violating therapist behaviours as well as other orientation and submission for approval. Second, some preferred a close patient–therapist alliance with therapist self-disclosure and reciprocity was a requirement for opening up; others required distance. Third, informants detached from experiential trauma work while engaging in joint reflection on post-trauma responses.

Conclusion

Informants were preoccupied with calibrating the emotional–relational landscape in session; we hypothesized that psychological insecurity and affective intolerance from CT limit their freedom to explore own in-session experiences.

Plain English summary

A patient perspective on individual ED treatment processes is scarce for difficult-to-treat eating disorders (EDs) with childhood trauma (CT). We therefore interviewed six poor long-term outcome inpatients through video-assisted recall about a self-selected therapy session. Patients’ covert in-session strategies included self-effacing behaviours in relation to their therapist, their respective preferences for closeness or distance to their therapist to be able to open up in session, and being more prone to engage in therapists’ reflective interventions rather than experiential. In sum, patients were preoccupied with calibrating the emotional–relational landscape between patient and therapist in session, which were strategies that went undetected. We hypothesized that a lack of psychological security and affective tolerance limit patients freedom to explore own experiences from the perspective of traumatic attachment and self-differentiation theory.

Background

Childhood trauma (CT) seems more common in eating disorders (EDs) than in the general population: prevalence rates range from 37 to 100% depending on CT definitions [1]. Compared to EDs without CT, the ED-CT link is associated with earlier ED onset, greater bingeing/purging, dissociation, suicidality, psychiatric comorbidity, and ED severity, alongside an increased probability for treatment drop-out [2,3,4,5,6,7]. ED treatments are lacking for this patient group [8, 9], and directions for integrating ED and trauma treatments do not exist [1].

CT is a risk factor for ED development, and trauma symptoms may reinforce the ED as ED behaviours may facilitate avoidance of distressing trauma symptoms, and thus decrease hyperarousal [10]. A network analysis of ED and PTSD bridge symptoms found associations between bingeing and irritability, body dissatisfaction and distressing dreams, and shape difficulties and concentration difficulties [11]. CT in EDs also associate with avoidant coping and ED symptoms in a dose–response manner [12], supporting that childhood sexual abuse with avoidant personality predict ED chronicity [13]. In fact, avoidance, rumination and suppression of emotional expression are all linked to increased ED severity. Little is, however, known about how ED patients modify their emotions in relation to others [14], which is relevant as therapists’ and patients’ moment-to-moment regulation strategies seem to interact [15].

Patients themselves similarly connected EDs with CT to emotional, interpersonal and self-image regulation difficulties in two patient-as-expert studies. Bingeing was perceived as a strategy for comfort and relieving trauma-related emotions, and purging following bingeing for weight control to avoid rejection and abandonment. Bingeing without purging was conversely perceived as protection from sexual abuse through weight gain [16, 17]. Our study added the repeated use of ED behaviours to build mastery and cure helplessness/emptiness, and the ED was conceptualised as a secure base and emotion regulation strategy in the absence of co-regulation abilities [17]. CT late effects with EDs thus seem pervasive beyond PTSD, at least for some, following complex PTSD (CPTSD; [18]).

Foreseeably, interpersonal problems and attachment insecurity may affect the ability to engage in productive working alliances [19], which both parties contribute to through constant negotiation of mutual goals, task collaboration, and emotional bond [20]. However, while the alliance quality has repeatedly proven to be a reliable predictor of moderate size for treatment outcome across disorders and orientations [21, 22], its role in ED treatment has been unclear [23, 24]. A meta-analysis, however, concluded that the alliance-outcome association was partially accounted for by early symptom reduction, as early ED symptom improvement predicted subsequent alliance quality and vice versa. The alliance’s impact on outcome also varied on a subgroup level, being stronger for younger patients (cf. older patients) with AN (cf. other EDs) and weaker for treatments with a strong behavioural component [25].

Quantification of the alliance is nevertheless problematic, as there may be qualitative differences to the constructs measured [22]. Notably, patients’ views as opposed to independent raters’ or therapists’ views hold strongest clinical validity [25] and little is known about the treatment processes that concur with non-responses or iatrogenic effects. Reflecting openly on unfavourable outcomes is relevant since understanding treatment failures may guide treatment modifications [26, 27]. Thus, patient perspective inquiries exploring features of unproductive alliances as well as productive ones merit attention.

Our patient-as-expert process-outcome study broached aspects of emotion and alliance experiencing as we compared change processes in inpatient ED treatment for good (n = 4) versus poor (n = 7) long-term outcomes. Salient processes for good outcomes were: fostering patient agency, engaging in trauma exposure and creating a balance between self-assertion and vulnerability within the therapist-patient dyad. Central processes in poor outcome patients were: orientation towards others’ needs, distrust in their own abilities and difficulties showing vulnerability, in addition to not addressing trauma and maintaining either a distanced or idealised therapist relationship [28]. Nevertheless, patients’ moment-to-moment experiences remain undisclosed, calling for research on treatment processes in relation to outcomes [29].

One strategy to disentangle such fine-grained processes is through Interpersonal Process Recall (IPR; [30]), which facilitates the retrieval of unspoken experiences through video-assisted recall. An early IPR study revealed outpatients’ (diagnoses unknown) general tendency to act deferentially towards their therapist (fear of criticising the therapist, eagerness to meet therapist expectations, and concern about the approach), and responses depending on what they feel they can disclose safely and whether they feel comfortable approaching inner experiences [31]. Another IPR study revealed that outpatients’ (diagnoses unknown) emotional disengagement (withdrawing, distancing, or lessening intensity) may be both productive and obstructive, e.g., protecting oneself from potentially painful content, choosing what to disclose to maintain safety, testing therapist responses to avoidance and vulnerability, and interrupting self-impeding patterns [32]. Yet another study revealed that patients with anxiety, depression, PTSD, and personality disorders experienced inner struggles when attempting to open up during the initial phases of affect-focused therapy, for example fearing the intensity and consequences of negative emotions, feeling disloyal to loved ones, insecurity about one’s right to share inner experiences, and being bodily stuck and unable to open up [33]. We believe that video-assisted recall is yet to be used to study treatment micro-processes in EDs and CT late effects.

Consequently, we aim to extend previous research by uncovering moment-to-moment processes in patients with EDs and CT late effects through the research question: How do ED treatment non-responders with CT late effects experience in-session processes within the context of the patient-therapist working alliance? Thus, by identifying covert processes from the patients’ vantage point, we aim to pinpoint therapeutic processes in poor long-term ED outcomes within the framework of an inpatient randomised controlled trial (RCT).

Method

Setting and overarching RCT

The study was conducted in a tertiary care psychiatric hospital in southeast Norway, treating various disorders. The hospital is part of the public health care system and admits patients on a nationwide basis free of charge. Our sample comprised of patients from a highly specialised eating disorder unit, and specifically those participating in an RCT comparing 13-week inpatient Compassion-Focused Therapy for EDs (CFT-E) with Cognitive-Behavioural Therapy for EDs (CBT-ED) in EDs with or without CT (Clinical trials: NCT02649114) with the assumption that CFT-E would be more beneficial for EDs with CT. The trial protocol is described in detail elsewhere [34]: the RCT findings are yet to be concluded. Our study targeted micro-processes within the patient-therapist dyad for poor long-term ED outcome inpatients with psychiatric sequelae from emotional, physical, and/or sexual CT.

Treatments

CBT-ED

CBT-ED [35] is the first choice ED treatment targeting cognitive-behavioural change and maintaining factors, e.g., disordered eating, compensatory beliefs about needing to control weight/food intake, and core beliefs about low self-worth. CBT-ED incorporates traditional CBT techniques such as graded exposure and cognitive restructuring alongside ED-specific targets such as dietary change. If late CT effects play a role in ED maintenance, imaginal exposure, and rescripting is incorporated. For more information on CBT-ED within the RCT, see trial protocol [34].

CFT-E

CFT-E [36] focuses on emotion regulation and shame and self-criticism as ED maintenance factors, which frequently occur in EDs with CT and also seem involved in the maintenance of post-traumatic sequelae [37, 38]. CFT-E aims to balance patients’ threats, soothing, and drive systems, and promote compassion for self and others. This is achieved through mindfulness and visualisation techniques and by giving and receiving compassionate support in groups. See trial protocol [34] for the dissemination of CFT in the current setting.

Diagnostic assessment

Experienced clinicians from a separate assessment team conducted MINI and SCID-II, and two psychology students blinded to group allocation and trained in diagnostic assessment conducted EDE-16. Therapists and participants were not blinded to group allocation, since the treatments in the RCT were of a psychological nature.

General psychopathology

The MINI International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI; [39]) is a diagnostic interview that assesses DSM-IV axis I disorders in adults, such as psychosis and depression. The MINI has good psychometric properties across treatment contexts [40, 41].

Personality disorders

The Structural Clinical Interview for DSM-IV axis II disorders (SCID-II; [42]) was used to assess personality traits. SCID-II includes 13 DSM-IV personality disorders. Patients were assessed using the standard categorical approach: they qualified for a personality disorder if they fulfilled a predetermined number of criteria.

Eating disorder

The Eating Disorder Examination Interview 16.0 (EDE-16; [43]) is a 42-item semi-structured interview that guided clinicians’ assessment of ED pathology during the previous four weeks (Restraint, Eating concern, Shape concern, Weight concern), summarised in a Global EDE score. Most items are rated on a 7-point Likert scale (0–6), except for the frequency of ED behaviours. The Norwegian EDE-16 has acceptable internal consistency and inter-rater reliability [44].

Self-report measures

Eating pathology

Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire 6.0 (EDE-Q; [45]) is a 28-item self-report questionnaire measuring the frequency and intensity of ED symptoms during the previous four weeks (e.g., “a definite fear of losing control over eating”). Symptoms are rated on a 7-point Likert scale, where a global score indicating ED severity is divided into four subscales: Restraint, Eating concern, Shape concern, and Weight concern. The Norwegian EDE-Q has shown acceptable internal consistency, reliability, and validity [46]. Cronbach’s alpha for our sample was 0.89 at assessment.

Childhood trauma

A 28-item short form of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ-SF; [47]) guided patient inclusion in the RCT (completed at assessment). CTQ-SF measures the severity of sexual, physical, and emotional abuse alongside physical and emotional neglect. Each subscale has five items graded on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = never true, 5 = very often true). CTQ has satisfactory psychometric properties across Norwegian samples [48]. Cronbach’s alpha for the total scale in our sample was 0.79.

Trauma symptoms

PTSD Symptom Scale Self-report version (PSS-SR; [49]) measures seventeen DSM-IV PTSD symptoms during the previous week, rated on a 4-point Likert scale (0 = not at all, 3 = always or five or more times weekly). A total score measures PTSD severity divided into three subscales assessing trauma-related hyperarousal, re-experiencing, and avoidance. The Norwegian PSS-SR has good concurrent validity and test–retest reliability [50]. Cronbach’s alpha for our sample at assessment was 0.92.

Interpersonal problems

The 64-item version of the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems (IIP-64; [51]) measured problematic interpersonal behaviours on a 5-point Likert scale (0 = not at all, 4 = extremely): 39 items measuring difficulties regarding others (It is hard for me to…) and 25 items concerning behaviours that patients feel they are doing too much regarding others (Things that you do too much). The subscales correspond to octants in a circumplex model: cold, vindictive, domineering, intrusive, self-sacrificing, overly accommodating, non-assertive, and socially inhibited. There are two dimensions to the model: agency (y-axis) and communion (x-axis). Norm scores are calculated to determine patients’ caseness for each variable [54], meaning that a total score exceeding 1 represents caseness. The psychometric properties of the Norwegian IIP-64 are acceptable [52], and Cronbach’s alpha for our sample at assessment was 0.85.

Therapeutic working alliance

The 12-item short form of the Working Alliance Inventory (WAI-SR; [53]) measures the patient-therapist quality during the previous week rated on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = never, 7 = always). Data were collected weekly during treatment starting the third week, and the scale was divided into three subscales based on Bordin’s [20] working alliance theory: agreeing on treatment goals (Goals), agreeing on tasks to achieve those goals (Tasks), and the quality of the patient-therapist relationship (Bond). The 3-factor model has demonstrated good psychometric properties, internal consistency, and convergent validity across countries and contexts [54, 55]. Cronbach’s alpha for our sample ranged from 0.75 to 0.93.

Case selection

All RCT participants from March 2016 to February 2017 who fulfilled the inclusion criteria of an ED diagnosis (DSM-5; [56]) with CT (CTQ) were considered eligible. To define accurate case determination for the RCT, the recommended CTQ scoring options by Walker and colleagues [57] was used. Threshold scores for each of the five subscales were based on receiver operating characteristic methods, with very good to excellent sensitivity and specificity (≥ 0.85) for each of the five subscales. Hence, patients scoring ≥ 8 on the sexual abuse, physical abuse, or physical neglect subscale or ≥ 10 on the emotional abuse subscale or ≥ 15 on the emotional neglect subscale were considered trauma patients in the RCT, thus eligible for participation in the study. Exclusion criteria were current psychosis, ongoing trauma (e.g., still being in an abusive relationship), or suicidality requiring extensive care, i.e., acute suicidality not manageable in settings that function on voluntary admissions.

A poor long-term outcome was defined as not meeting either full or partial recovery at 1-year follow-up, defining full recovery as (a) not fulfilling an ED diagnosis, (b) no restraint, bingeing, and purging for 3 months, (c) BMI > 18.5, and (d) EDE-Q subscale scores within 1 SD of age-matched community norms. For partial recovery (d), psychological ED components remained [58]. Criteria a and b were assessed through EDE-Q scores and clinicians’ opinions, and Norwegian EDE-Q norms were compared against Australian age-matched norms to ensure correspondence [59, 60]. Accordingly, 18 RCT inpatients were eligible for the current study during this period. Two declined participation, and four were not introduced to the study for unknown reasons, resulting in 12 possible informants. Technical challenges and poor video sound quality then ruled out five informants, narrowing the sample down to seven possible informants who had recordings of acceptable quality. Of these, six had poor outcomes at the 1-year follow-up according to the aforementioned outcome criteria, and were chosen for analysis (Table 1), meaning that data saturation was not involved in determining sample size, but rather, informants were selected for analysis post-interview.

Table 1 Case selection of non-recovery based on assessment at 1-year follow-up

Participants

Informants

Six white Norwegian females with EDs and CT and an average age of 40.2 years (SD = 9.8, range 28–59) were interviewed post-treatment (three CBT-ED, three CFT-E). Five (83%) had experienced at least three trauma types, most commonly emotional neglect (n = 6, 100%), followed by emotional abuse (n = 5, 83%), physical neglect (n = 5, 83%), physical abuse (n = 3, 50%), and sexual abuse (n = 2, 35%). Average ED duration was 28.2 years (SD = 6.4, range 14–54), with 12 years as the mean age for ED onset (SD = 1.6, range 6–17). Patients had on average been in treatment for 11.2 years prior to admission (SD = 4.0, range 1–26) (Table 2).

Table 2 Patient pre-treatment characteristics and treatment in the RCT

Therapists

Five white female therapists (four clinical psychologists, one psychiatrist) specialised in ED treatment for 5.8 years on average participated (Table 2). They were trained in the respective treatments and received regular supervision from the primary treatment developers as part of the RCT. Neither therapists nor supervisors were part of the research team.

Researchers

Our team comprised white Norwegian-speaking psychologists with clinical/research experience: a female clinical psychologist, a female associate professor with ED expertise, a male professor in clinical psychology, and a female associate professor in clinical psychology. We have competence in CBT, CFT, existential therapy, systemic and narrative/language-based, and affect/emotion-focused models.

Procedure and data collection

A semi-structured interview guide was developed according to Interpersonal Process Recall (IPR; [30]), where informants were prompted through open-ended, exploratory questions (Additional file 1). IPR is an audio-or video-assisted recall method that aims to facilitate the retrieval of “conscious yet unspoken experiences” [61, p. 18], in this case, covert treatment processes. The methodology is a joint process of meaning-making as the interviewer and informant together attempt to make the implicit explicit through dialogue. This allows for reflexive co-analysis beyond informants’ actual experiences there and then while removing unnecessary constraints to discovery [62].

Individual therapy sessions took place twice weekly as part of the 13-week inpatient treatment; they were recorded and stored on a local server at the treatment facility for fidelity purposes in the RCT. Informants were briefed about the research project by their therapists as they approached discharge, and the first author informed those interested in the project and asked for informed written consent. The first author viewed and summarised all available video recordings prior to each interview and briefly presented their content to the informants at the start of each interview. Next, informants were instructed to select one session that they considered important regarding experiencing therapeutic work and collaboration processes, as best as they could consider the time interval between session and interview. The IPR interview was originally designed to be conducted within 48 h of the video-recorded session, but has been practiced at longer intervals [63, 64]. In our study, time intervals were compromised for practicality reasons and took place on one occasion, on average, 170 days post-discharge (SD = 56, range 11–339). Due to the evocative nature of the IPR method, in which informants are exposed to their previous sessions through the videos, these time intervals were not considered to threaten the validity of informants’ recalls of their own experiences [65].

Interview data was collected between November 2016 and February 2017, meaning that interviews took place before case selection of poor outcome cases at 1-year follow-up. The six selected videos occurred between Sessions 5 and 22 out of 26 sessions over 13 weeks (two weekly, Table 2). The first author replayed the selected video on a 13″ screen laptop computer to stimulate recall and shared exploration, and the informants were instructed to pause the video whenever they could recall any significant experiences that they wanted to explore further. Four interviews were conducted during post-treatment in informants’ homes, one at the treatment facility during 1-year follow-up, and one at the University of Oslo, due to informants’ wishes.

Data analysis

Interviews lasting between 97 and 140 min were audiotaped, and both video-recorded sessions and interviews were transcribed verbatim, creating parallel conversations. Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis (IPA; [66]) with theoretical roots in phenomenology, hermeneutics, and idiography was deemed especially suitable since our data was rich and complex, and concerned the personal experiences of a small sample. In practice, IPA researchers should try to put aside their taken-for-granted assumptions about the world to the extent possible to capture the phenomenon under study (bracketing) and focus on making sense and interpreting informants’ sense-making of their life worlds (double hermeneutics) while protecting the nuances of each informant’s narrative (idiography). In IPA, the individual has precedence over the general, and accordingly, themes were first identified in a case-to-case manner to capture individual nuances. Master Themes (MTs) resulted from the further analysis that aimed to capture commonalities across the sample, and therefore represent abstractions of individual themes as opposed to themes that were salient across the whole sample. We added the “coding for process” element from Corbin & Strauss’s [67] version of Grounded Theory (GT), meaning that we explored patterns and variation in patient-therapist interactions across dyads, or “how the main issues /…/ are handled through action and interaction by participants, and how change in response relate to changes in condition” [67, p. 293].

All analyses were conducted in Norwegian. The first author read through the transcripts while listening to recordings to immerse in data. Transcripts were then analysed separately in a case-to-case manner focusing on processes, and the first author discussed emerging themes with the fourth author. Specifically, each subsequent interview was sensitised by previous interviews, meaning that we sought to label themes following previously labelled themes. The second and third authors then read through the transcripts to become familiar with the data, and they were presented with interpretive higher-order themes by the first author, which were concluded conjointly as deviations were addressed. Finally, quotes were translated by the first author with a double master’s degree in English and Nordic languages.

Trustworthiness

The trustworthiness of the study was addressed [68]. Subjectivity and researcher bias were managed by making assumptions overt through ongoing reflexive discussions, which was necessary since two members of our team had central roles in the RCT. The fourth author with the most experience in qualitative research audited the process, and we used purposeful sampling to identify good exemplars of the phenomenon under study and multiple data sources (interview, self-report, video recordings) to obtain thick descriptions. Also, informants had unlimited time to express themselves, and four were interviewed in their homes, which enhanced the interpretive status of the evidence. Data immersion from data collection to communication of results helped to achieve a deeper understanding of the data and ensured the groundedness of the themes.

Ethical considerations

It was explicitly communicated verbally and in writing that participation was voluntary, that participants were free to decline at any time without giving a reason, and that non-participation would not influence possibilities for future treatment. Due to the presumed vulnerability of the patient group in combination with sensitive material in the videotapes (e.g., trauma exposure and viewing ones’ body) that could potentially evoke difficult emotions or heighten the risk of self-destructive behaviours, the interviewer was available for contact via phone and e-mail before and after the interviews. We established that informants had therapists locally at the time of the interview to ensure their safety. All procedures were approved by the Norwegian Regional Committee for Medical Research Ethics (nr 2015/2160) and conducted according to the Helsinki declaration.

Results

All informants scored within the clinical range on EDE-Q (M = 4.5; SD = 0.7, range 3.5–5.1) pre-treatment. Four (all except Alice and Maria) also scored within the clinical range PTSD range (M = 20.5; SD = 11.0, range 6–30). IIP-64 tendencies leaned towards the non-agentic pole with mixed scores for the communion dimension (Fig. 1), except for one outlier (Alice) who did not score for caseness on either PTSD or interpersonal problems. WAI weekly subscale scores ranged from 1 to 7 (Additional file 2: Fig. S1, Additional file 3: Fig. S2, Additional file 4: Fig. S3). The transcriptions rendered 47 excerpts relevant to the study’s aim; in 30 of the cases, the informant paused the recording.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Individual IIP-64 scores at the start of treatment

Informants expressed interest in the video; however, several mentioned initial reflective difficulties due to being visually exposed to their weight/shape, and overall, they needed prompting to work their way into the IPR method and the video. The overarching themes self-effacement, regulating closeness, and distance to the therapist to be able to open up and emotional avoidance during trauma work across five informants are illustrated below, followed by the narrative of a diverging case (Alice), who described agency and emotional experiencing while supported by the therapist. All themes are exemplified in Table 3, in which patients’ accounts are displayed regarding each therapy segment.

Table 3 Example excerpts

Overarching themes

Eleven themes (A–I) were constructed from interpretations of data and abstracted into three Master Themes (MTs, Table 4). Notably, one informant (Alice) diverged from the others’ accounts; hence, her theme is not included in the cross-case analysis (see Tables 3, 5).

Table 4 Themes and master themes across cases
Table 5 Individual patterns of themes

MT1: P managing interactions with T through self-effacing and submissive behaviours

MT1 comprises six themes (A–F) on patients’ self-effacing and submissive interactions with the therapist (Elizabeth, Sandra, and Helena). Patient responses to therapist behaviours perceived as criticising, violating, or forceful were submission, passivity, pretending to participate, and withdrawing self-assertion, from fearing negative consequences. Elizabeth explains that “I feel rebuked /…/ insecure about whether or not to speak my opinion/…/ scared of getting hit verbally” (A), and Helena states that “I say that clearly there and then, but then I laugh /…/ because I don’t want to hurt her feelings” (E). Other salient themes concerned other-orienting towards the therapist’s presumed agenda due to a fear of failing therapy, as Sandra states, “I know what to answer, but I don’t dare saying it /…/ the constant fear of saying something wrong” (B) or submitting for approval as Helena states “I’m actually sitting there lying/…/ trying to please her /…/ be good, and say what I’m supposed to” (D).

MT2: P preferring either closeness or distance to T to be able to open up

MT2 comprises three themes (G–I) centering on patients’ preferences of closeness or distance to the therapist to be able to open up, represented by Elizabeth, Sandra, Martha, and Maria. The two closeness-seeking strategies concerned patients’ positive responses to therapist self-disclosure that was perceived as blurring patient-therapist boundaries (H) and the positive impact of the therapist’s reciprocity and intonation regarding increased trust and opening up (I). The former was expressed by Elizabeth: “even though I understand that it’s a bad idea to become best friends with your therapist, it is not bad /…/ I’m more interested, it changed instantly when she started talking about herself” (H). The latter was exemplified by Martha, who stated that her therapist was «even more on my inside than she was supposed to at times too, inside of what I was telling her, and I guess that made me feel that she really meant it” and that she “trusted and trust her completely” (I). Maria, on the contrary, preferred a distanced patient-therapist relationship, describing it as “so much more difficult being open to other people” and the therapist as “a person that I, don’t have to adhere to in my daily life” (G).

MT3: P joining T’s reflective interventions and detaching from T’s experiential techniques during trauma work

MT3 comprises two themes (J–K) on patients’ access to different experiential levels, as they seemed more inclined to engage in reflective interventions than those of emotion or experience (Elizabeth, Sandra, Martha, Maria). First, patients expressed emotional detachment when overwhelmed by interventions; for example, Maria described that “everything becomes dark” and not having “much contact with the body” during experiential trauma interventions, while at the same time finding it beneficial to talk about post-traumatic reactions (J). Second, informants expressed a need for the therapist to help create logic between past and current experiences. Martha, for instance, explained how it “feels good to go through things step-by-step /…/ to hear “that, that, and that… equals this”./…/ you do know it, of course, yes, but the body doesn’t want to, the feeling and, really, the whole body is protecting itself” (K).

Diverging theme

T deepening and containing P’s emotional experiences in a composed manner; P opening up and exploring; P increased trust and stronger bond with T

One informant (Alice) came forth with a differing narrative as her accounts included emotional exploration and expression that she perceived the therapist to contain, which in turn strengthened their relationship and provided a stronger base for exploring. She described that her therapist “got something out of me that meant something for the relationship that we share /…/ showed me that she understood, what, what’s the issue, and how that’s an issue” and that “it came to that I worked more with what she, uhm, she tried to evoke”. She also added that she felt “listened to, understood, getting in contact with my tears, and she’s still composed and meeting my eyes when I look at her and, yes, available and helpful even though I started crying”. This narrative contrasts the others since Alice describes agency (cf. self-effacement) and emotional experiencing (cf. avoidance) under therapist guidance.

Discussion

This study provides insights into how six non-recovered inpatients with EDs and comorbid CT late effects experienced in-session therapeutic processes post-treatment. The Core Master Theme encompassed covert strategies of self-effacement (three informants), preferring either therapist closeness or distance to open up (four informants) and more easily adhering to reflective than experiential interventions (four informants). MTs occurred across both treatment models and were central for five informants, whereas Alice’s descriptions diverged from the general cross-case pattern. Since IPA is interpretive in nature and meant to capture idiographic nuances of the accounts, themes were at times represented by one informant only (Tables 4, 5).

The first MT, P managing interactions with T through self-effacement and submission, entailed passive, submissive patient responses (suppressing anger/contempt, withdrawing assertion) to therapist behaviours that were perceived as violating or forceful. They also included patients’ other orientation that sprung from a need for therapist approval or avoiding failure. Since patients pretended to participate, strategies seemed to occur outside therapists’ awareness: at least they were not addressed explicitly in session. Findings converged with real-time self-reported interpersonal problems pre-treatment, with general tendencies towards the non-agentic IIP-64 pole (Fig. 1), and with mid-range level treatment processes that include patients’ focus on others’ needs [28]. Findings also align with patients’ proneness to deferential acts, e.g., fearing to criticise the therapist and eagerness to fulfil therapist expectations, and responses based on what they feel they can disclose safely [31]. Alice diverged from this pattern as self-effacement was absent in her descriptions, which overlapped with her pre-treatment IIP-64 scores showing no caseness for interpersonal problems (Fig. 1).

The second MT reflecting patients’ preferences of either therapist closeness or distance to open up included two themes that referred to (a) therapist self-disclosure and (b) therapist reciprocity and intonation, with closeness and blurred patient–therapist relationships as prerequisites for opening up (Elizabeth, Sandra, Martha). On the contrary, Maria preferred distance from her therapist due to difficulties being open when feeling too close. Notably, Maria scored > 5 for WAI emotional bond during treatment week five, with a steep decline the following week and persisting until discharge, whereas Elizabeth reported low scores with a slight increase throughout. Martha and Sandra instead reported high WAI emotional bond scores during the treatment course (Additional file 4: Fig. S3). This indicates that patients’ preferences for either pole on the closeness/distance continuum in our study largely, but not unanimously, concurred with patients’ self-reported real-time emotional bonds with their therapists, as measured by WAI. Also, since all informants had poor long-term outcomes and some had high WAI emotional bond scores which would normally relate to good outcomes, it raises new hypotheses on the ED alliance-outcome association. One explanation for our findings may be that the emotional bond functions differently regarding a treatment’s goals and tasks, aligning with research showing that different types of emotional bonds may function differently in different treatment models in producing favourable outcomes [69, 70].

Findings align with traumatically attached people being more inclined to experience internal struggles between the drive to connect and the drive to fight or flee, therefore presenting with either dismissing or suspicious preoccupied tendencies with fear of abandonment or rejection [71]. We thus suggest that MT2 processes relate to differentiation dysfunction, where healthy differentiation entails parallel developmental processes of proximity seeking and increased autonomy. The ultimate goal of these parallel processes is psychological security [72 cited in 73], meaning the “freedom to explore the inner and outer world” (p. 859), i.e., not having to compensate for relational difficulties. Insecure attachment developing in childhood instead creates obstacles to healthy differentiation, leading to an overemphasis on the development of either closeness-seeking or distancing, in line with MT2 descriptions.

The third MT revealed that patients more readily engaged with reflective verbal interventions and detached from experiential techniques during trauma work. We suggest that this relates to emotion intolerance and dysregulation that may follow traumatic attachment through patients’ under-/overactivity of the stress response system. A resilient nervous system that recovers easily from distress (sympathetic activation) or boredom (parasympathetic activation) is built from consistent interactive regulation in early attachment relations, with development of self-regulation over time. When attachment figures engage in interactions that alarm the child (acting frightening/frightened), a flexible window of affect tolerance fails to develop [71]. In agreement, disorganised attachment significantly predicts dissociation, Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD), and Dissociative Identity Disorder (DID) [74], which also aligns with the pervasiveness of EDs with late CT effects resembling CPTSD [17], and the fact that dissociation seems more common in EDs with CT than in the general population and other disorders [75]. Alice who diverged from this general pattern instead described an ability to focus on the exploration and deepening of emotions, while being contained and supported by her therapist. The combination of emotional arousal and reflective exploration is generally associated with favourable outcomes across disorders and treatments [74], but did not, however, seem to be sufficient for full recovery according to established outcome criteria ([58]; Table 1).

We suggest that differentiation issues, self-effacement and emotion dysregulation interplay unfavourably in treatment; our clinically diverging case adds information to the general pattern and raises further questions. Notably, Alice differed from the others since she showed neither pre-treatment caseness for PTSD (PSS-SR) nor interpersonal problems (IIP-64), and she did not fulfil an ED diagnosis at 1-year follow-up. This suggests that she was approaching ED recovery, although self-reported residual restraint made her disqualify as partially recovered since only cognitive residual symptoms are allowed for partial recovery ([58]; Table 1). Findings underscore that CT in itself does not equal post-traumatic sequelae, but that mediators such as PTSD, emotion dysregulation, and/or interpersonal difficulties may be at play to complicate and prolong the treatment course. This concurs with research suggesting that PTSD, emotional dysregulation, interpersonal difficulties, and avoidant personality styles mediate CT’s impact on ED development, severity, and chronicity [2, 10, 12, 13].

Clinical implications

This study targets the intersection of relational functioning and lack of long-term change in EDs with CT, and may be immediately clinically relevant since it concerns actual real-life patient–therapist interactions. Covert preoccupation with navigating the therapeutic relationship were central in the narratives of patients with EDs and CT and pre-treatment caseness for ED, interpersonal problems, and post-trauma symptoms. Awareness around patient characteristics may therefore alert clinicians to explicitly address inter- and intrapersonal processes that are potentially impeding treatment progress. Undisclosed under/overactivated states may for instance have important consequences for trauma exposure treatment since therapists risk that patients engage in covert experiential avoidance and end up without treatment effect.

Study limitations and future research

This study has some limitations. First, informants were purposefully selected based on outcome assessments at 1-year follow-up, and since we were not blinded to outcomes, this may have coloured our interpretations to emphasise unfavourable aspects of the process accounts. Second, although findings highlighted processes in a patients with poor long-term outcomes, we cannot claim these processes to produce poor long-term ED outcomes, and we call for caution about any causality claims. Also, the lack of control group made us unaware of whether the same processes occur also in favourable long-term outcomes. Third, generalisability is limited due to the small sample size, although we do recognise patients’ accounts as important in generating novel information in line with an explorative aim and methodology that do not make use of predetermined categories, and thus is suitable for complex, understudied topics. Fourth, patients’ ability to express themselves about complex processes may vary, and to help informants counteract possible biases, we used IPR to focus informants’ attention on their own mental processes and provide suitable stimuli as recommended [64]. Fifth, the less-than-ideal and varying time intervals between treatment and IPR interviews may have impacted our findings through which data was ultimately obtained. However, although interpretations of past events are subject to change over time, descriptions also seem to mature and enrich over time [77]. Moreover, the evocative nature of IPR likely helps informants come into contact with and recall experiences better than regular interviews, where time intervals may be a more crucial issue. Importantly, we prioritized travelling to the informants if they wished to be interviewed at home, and thus, we compromised this time interval to capture rich data from these often poorly functioning patients, who may not have been able to participate in the study otherwise. Sixth, informants’ interpersonal styles, such as self-reported non-agentic tendencies (non-assertive, overly accommodating, socially inhibited), may have played out during the interviews. This potential source of error may, however, have been partly counteracted by the interviewer being a licenced psychologist with clinical experience.

Accordingly, we propose that future research explore moment-to-moment interactions for good outcomes to disentangle processes associated with a successful treatment course. Also, by continuing sampling and through other analytical methods focusing on theory building [68], it is possible to construct contextualized, explanatory models. In order to make inferences, larger samples and repeated measures designs could test models within and across treatment contexts and shed further light on how variables interrelate over time.

Conclusion

Our findings provide novel insights in terms of plausible ED treatment modifications, as we revealed that a sample of poor outcome ED patients CT were predominantly preoccupied with calibrating the emotional–relational landscape of the patient-therapist relationship in-session; we hypothesize that differentiation dysfunction, psychological insecurity and affective intolerance limited patients’ freedom to explore own experiences. One remaining question, however, is whether working on patients’ relational difficulties has spillover effects on the ED, or whether relational issues are hindering factors that need to be eliminated for patients to benefit from more targeted ED treatments.

Availability of data and materials

This study is situated within a randomized trial (Clinical trials: NCT02649114), using interview data as primary material. The data are not publicly available due to privacy or ethical restrictions, but may be available on request from the corresponding author.

References

  1. Trim JG, Galowski TE, Wagner A, Brewerton TD. Treating eating disorder-post-traumatic stress disorder patients: a synthesis of the literature and new treatment directions. In: Anderson LK, Murray SB, Kaye WH, editors. Clinical handbook of complex and atypical eating disorders. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2018. p. 40–59.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Brewerton TD. Eating disorders, trauma, and comorbidity: focus on PTSD. Eat Disord. 2007;15(4):285–304. https://doi.org/10.1080/10640260701454311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Brewerton TD. An overview of trauma-informed care and practice for eating disorders. J Aggress Maltreatment Trauma. 2019;28(4):445–62. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926771.2018.1532940.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Castellini G, Lelli L, Cassioli E, Ciampi E, Zamponi F, Campone B, et al. Different outcomes, psychopathological features, and comorbidities in patients with eating disorders reporting childhood abuse: a 3-year follow-up study. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2018;26(3):217–29. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2586.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Mahon J, Bradley SN, Harvey PK, Winston AP, Palmer RL. Childhood trauma has dose–effect relationship with dropping out from psychotherapeutic treatment for bulimia nervosa: a replication. Int J Eat Disord. 2001;30(2):138–48. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.1066.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Molendijk ML, Hoek HW, Brewerton TD, Elzinga BM. Childhood maltreatment and eating disorder pathology: a systematic review and dose-response meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2017;19:1–15. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716003561.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Rabito-Alcón MF, Baile JI, Vanderlinden J. Child trauma experiences and dissociative symptoms in women with eating disorders: case-control study. Children (Basel). 2020;7(12):274. https://doi.org/10.3390/children7120274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. National Guideline Alliance (UK). Eating disorders: recognition and treatment. London: National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (UK); 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Wilson GT, Grilo CM, Vitousek KM. Psychological treatment of eating disorders. Am Psychol. 2007;62(3):199–216. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.62.3.199.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Trottier K, MacDonald DE. Update on psychological trauma, other severe adverse experiences and eating disorders: state of the research and future research directions. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2017;19(8):45. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-017-0806-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Vanzhula IA, Calebs B, Fewell L, Levinson CA. Illness pathways between eating disorder and post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms: understanding comorbidity with network analysis. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2019;27(2):147–60. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2634.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Rosenbaum DL, White KS, Artime TM. Coping with childhood maltreatment: avoidance and eating disorder symptoms. J Health Psychol. 2021;26(14):2832–40. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105320937068.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Vrabel KR, Hoffart A, Rø O, Martinsen EW, Rosenvinge JH. Co-occurrence of avoidant personality disorder and child sexual abuse predicts poor outcome in long-standing eating disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2010;119(3):623–9. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019857.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Christensen KA, Haynos AF. A theoretical review of interpersonal emotion regulation in eating disorders: enhancing knowledge by bridging interpersonal and affective dysfunction. J Eat Disord. 2020;8:21. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-020-00298-0.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Soma CS, Baucom BRW, Xiao B, Butner JE, Hilpert P, Narayanan S, et al. Coregulation of therapist and client emotion during psychotherapy. Psychother Res. 2020;30(5):591–603. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2019.1661541.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Moulding N. “It Wasn’t About Being Slim”: understanding eating disorders in the context of abuse. Violence Against Women. 2015;21(12):1456–80. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077801215596243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Olofsson ME, Oddli HW, Vrabel KR, Hoffart A. In solitude is safeness: a patient perspective on eating disorders in the context of multiple childhood trauma. Nord Psychol. 2020;73(1):29-42. https://doi.org/10.1080/19012276.2020.1762714.

  18. World Health Organization. The ICD-11 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Castonguay LG, Constantino MJ, Holtforth MG. The working alliance: where are we and where should we go? Psychotherapy (Chic). 2006;43(3):271–9. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-3204.43.3.271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Bordin E. The generalizability of the psychoanalytic concept of the working alliance. Psychotherapy. 1979;16:252–60. https://doi.org/10.1037/h008588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Flückiger C, Rubel J, Del Re AC, Horvath AO, Wampold BE, Crits-Christoph P, et al. The reciprocal relationship between alliance and early treatment symptoms: a two-stage individual participant data meta-analysis. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2020;88(9):829–43. https://doi.org/10.1037/ccp0000594.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Flückiger C, Del Re AC, Wampold BE, Horvath AO. The alliance in adult psychotherapy: a meta-analytic synthesis. Psychotherapy (Chic). 2018;55(4):316–40. https://doi.org/10.1037/pst0000172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Antoniou P, Cooper M. Psychological treatments for eating disorders: what is the importance of the quality of the therapeutic alliance for outcomes? Couns Psychol Rev. 2013;28(4):34–46.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Brown A, Mountford VA, Waller G. Is the therapeutic alliance overvalued in the treatment of eating disorders? Int J Eat Disord. 2013;46(8):779–82. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Graves TA, Tabri N, Thompson-Brenner H, Franko DL, Eddy KT, Bourion-Bedes S, et al. A meta-analysis of the relation between therapeutic alliance and treatment outcome in eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord. 2017;50(4):323–40. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22672.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Dimidjian S, Hollon SD. What can be learned when empirically supported treatments fail? Cogn Behav Pract. 2011;18(3):303–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpra.2011.02.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Lambert MJ. What have we learned about treatment failure in empirically supported treatments? Some suggestions for practice. Cogn Behav Pract. 2011;18(3):413–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpra.2011.02.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Olofsson ME, Oddli HW, Hoffart A, Eielsen HP, Vrabel KR. Change processes related to long-term outcomes in eating disorders with childhood trauma: An explorative qualitative study. J Couns Psychol. 2020;67(1):51–65. https://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000375.

  29. Norcross JC, Wampold BE. Evidence-based therapy relationships: research conclusions and clinical practices. Psychotherapy (Chic). 2011;48(1):98–102. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Kagan N, Kagan H. Interpersonal process recall. In: Dowrick PW, editor. Practical guide to using video in the behavioural sciences. Oxford: Wiley; 1991. p. 221–30.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Rennie DL. Clients’ deference in psychotherapy. J Couns Psychol. 1994;41(4):427–37. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.41.4.427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Frankel ZE, Levitt HM. Clients’ experiences of disengaged moments in psychotherapy: a grounded theory analysis. J Contemp Psychother. 2009;39(3):171–86. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10879-008-9087-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Kleiven GS, Hjeltnes A, Råbu M, Moltu C. Opening up: clients’ inner struggles in the initial phase of therapy. Front Psychol. 2020;11:591146. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.591146.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Vrabel KR, Wampold B, Quintana DS, Goss K, Waller G, Hoffart A. The Modum-ED trial protocol: comparing compassion-focused therapy and cognitive–behavioral therapy in treatment of eating disorders with and without childhood trauma: protocol of a randomized trial. Front Psychol. 2019;10:1638. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01638.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Waller G, Corstorphine E, Cordery H, Hinrichsen H, Lawson R, Mountford V. Cognitive–behavioral therapy for the eating disorders: a comprehensive treatment guide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Goss K, Allan S. Compassion focused therapy for eating disorders. Int J Cogn Ther. 2010;3(2):141–58. https://doi.org/10.1521/ijct.2010.3.2.141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Harman R, Lee D. The role of shame and self-critical thinking in the development and maintenance of current threat in post-traumatic stress disorder. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2010;17(1):13–24. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.636.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Øktedalen T. Are trauma-related shame and guilt part of the PTSD construct? (Dissertation). Oslo University; 2015.

  39. Lecrubier Y, Sheehan DV, Weiller E, Amorim P, Bonora I, Sheehan H, et al. The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): a short diagnostic structured interview: reliability and validity according to the CIDI. Eur Psychiatry. 2009;12(5):224–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0924-9338(97)83296-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Harnett Sheehan K, Janavs J, Weiller E, Keskiner A, et al. The validity of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) according to the SCID-P and its reliability. Eur Psychiatry. 1997;12(5):232–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Mordal J, Gundersen O, Bramness JG. Norwegian version of the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview: feasibility, acceptability and test-retest reliability in an acute psychiatric ward. Eur Psychiatry. 2010;25(3):172–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2009.02.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. First MB, Gibbon M, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Benjamin LS. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders, (SCID-II). Psychotherapy. 1997;55(4):316–40. https://doi.org/10.1037/pst0000172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Fairburn C. Cognitive behaviour therapy and eating disorders. New York: Guildford Press; 1998.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Reas DL, Wisting L, Kapstad H, Lask B. Convergent validity of the eating disorder examination and the eating disorder examination-questionnaire among university women in Norway. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2011;19(4):357–61. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.1068.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Cooper Z, Cooper PJ, Fairburn CG. The validity of the eating disorder examination and its subscales. Br J Psychiatry. 1989;154:807–12. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.154.6.807.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Rø Ø, Reas DL, Stedal K. Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire (EDE-Q) in Norwegian adults: discrimination between female controls and eating disorder patients. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2015;23(5):408–12. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2372.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Bernstein DP, Stein JA, Newcomb MD, Walker E, Pogge D, Ahluvalia T, Stokes J, Handelsman L, Medrano M, Desmond D, Zule W. Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child Abuse Negl. 2003;27(2):169–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0145-2134(02)00541-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Dovran A, Winje D, Overland SN, Breivik K, Arefjord K, Dalsbø AS, Jentoft MB, Hansen AL, Waage L. Psychometric properties of the Norwegian version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire in high-risk groups. Scand J Psychol. 2013;54(4):286–91. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12052.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Foa EB, Riggs DS, Dancu CV, Rothbaum BO. Reliability and validity of a brief instrument for assessing post-traumatic stress disorder. J Trauma Stress. 1993;6(4):459–73. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.2490060405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Langkaas TF, Hoffart A, Øktedalen T, Ulvenes PG, Hembree EA, Smucker M. Exposure and non-fear emotions: a randomized controlled study of exposure-based and rescripting-based imagery in PTSD treatment. Behav Res Ther. 2017;97:33–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2017.06.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Horowitz LM. Interpersonal foundations of psychopathology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2004. https://doi.org/10.1037/10727-000.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  52. Bjerke E. Interpersonal problems among psychiatric outpatients. A study of interpersonal problems and their associations with symptoms and forms of self-relatedness (Dissertation). Oslo University; 2016.

  53. Hatcher RL, Gillaspy A. Development and validation of a revised short version of the working alliance inventory. Psychother Res. 2006;16(1):12–25. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503300500352500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Falkenström F, Hatcher RL, Holmqvist R. Confirmatory factor analysis of the patient version of the Working Alliance Inventory-Short Form Revised. Assessment. 2015;22(5):581–93. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191114552472.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Munder T, Wilmers F, Leonhart R, Linster HW, Barth J. Working Alliance Inventory-Short Revised (WAI-SR): psychometric properties in outpatients and inpatients. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2010;17(3):231–9. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.658.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. DSM-V. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. 5th ed. Philadelphia: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  57. Walker EA, Gelfand A, Katon WJ, Koss MP, Von Korff M, Bernstein D, Russo J. Adult health status of women with histories of childhood abuse and neglect. Am J Med. 1999;107(4):332–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0002-9343(99)00235-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Bardone-Cone AM, Harney MB, Maldonado CR, Lawson MA, Robinson DP, Smith R, Tosh A. Defining recovery from an eating disorder: conceptualization, validation, and examination of psychosocial functioning and psychiatric comorbidity. Behav Res Ther. 2010;48(3):194–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Mond JM, Hay PJ, Rodgers B, Owen C. Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire (EDE-Q): norms for young adult women. Behav Res Ther. 2006;44(1):53–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2004.12.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Rø O, Reas DL, Lask B. Norms for the Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire among female university students in Norway. Nord J Psychiatry. 2010;64(6):428–32. https://doi.org/10.3109/08039481003797235.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Larsen D, Flesaker K, Stege R. Qualitative interviewing using interpersonal process recall: investigating internal experiences during professional-client conversations. Int J Qual. 2008;7(1):18–37. https://doi.org/10.1177/160940690800700102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Janusz B, Peräkylä A. Quality in conversation analysis and interpersonal process recall. Qual Res Psychol. 2020;18(3):426–49. https://doi.org/10.1080/14780887.2020.1780356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Engholm Hedegaard A. Supervisionsalliancen (Dissertation). Aalborg University; 2016.

  64. Macaskie J, Lees J, Freshwater D. Talking about talking: interpersonal process recall as an intersubjective approach to research. Psychodyn Pract. 2015;21(3):1–15. https://doi.org/10.1080/14753634.2015.1042517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Trnka R, Smelik V. Elimination of bias in introspection: methodological advances, refinements, and recommendations. New Idea Psych. 2020;56:100753.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Smith JA, Flowers P, Larkin M. Interpretative phenomenological analysis. London: SAGE; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Corbin J, Strauss A. Basics of qualitative research: techniques and procedures for developing grounded theory. 4th ed. Thousand Oaks: SAGE; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  68. Morrow SL. Quality and trustworthiness in qualitative research in counselling psychology. J Couns Psychol. 2005;52(2):250–60. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.52.2.250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Ulvenes PG, Berggraf L, Hoffart A, Stiles TC, Svartberg M, McCullough L, Wampold BE. Different processes for different therapies: therapist actions, therapeutic bond, and outcome. Psychotherapy (Chic). 2012;49(3):291–302. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027895.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Hatcher RL, Barends AW. How a return to theory could help alliance research. Psychotherapy (Chic). 2006;43(3):292–9. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-3204.43.3.292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Fisher J. Attachment as a sensorimotor experience: the use of sensorimotor therapy. New Dir Psychother Relat Psychoanal. 2011;5:99–107.

    Google Scholar 

  72. Grossmann K, Grossmann KE, Kindler H, Zimmermann P. A wider view of attachment and exploration: the influence of mothers and fathers on the development of psychological security from infancy to young adulthood. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of attachment: theory, research, and clinical applications. New York: The Guilford Press; 2008. p. 857–79.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Wennerberg T. Själv och tillsammans. Stockholm: Natur & Kultur; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  74. Pascual-Leone A, Yeryomenko N. The client “experiencing” scale as a predictor of treatment outcomes: a meta-analysis on psychotherapy process. Psychother Res. 2017;27(6):653–65. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2016.1152409.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Lyons-Ruth K, Dutra L, Schuder MR, Bianchi I. From infant attachment disorganization to adult dissociation: relational adaptations or traumatic experiences? Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2006;29(1):63–86, viii. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psc.2005.10.011.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Vanderlinden J, Palmisano G. Trauma and eating disorders: the state of the art. In: Seubert AJ, Virdi P, editors. Trauma-informed approaches to eating disorders. New York: Springer Publishing Company; 2018. p. 15–32.

    Google Scholar 

  77. Nilsson K, Abrahamsson E, Torbiornsson A, Hägglöf B. Causes of adolescent onset anorexia nervosa: patient perspectives. Eat Disord. 2007;15(2):125–33. https://doi.org/10.1080/10640260701190642.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank patients and staff from the Modum Bad eating disorder unit, and assistants Maja Kise and Kristin Heen Halvorsen for processing the primary data.

Funding

The study was funded by University of Oslo and Modum Bad Research Institute.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

MEO, HWO and KV are responsible for the study design, collecting and analyzing data, and drafting and revising the manuscript. AH participated in data analysis, and drafting and revising the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Malin E. Olofsson.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All procedures were approved by the Norwegian Regional Committee for Medical Research Ethics (2015/2160) and conducted according to the Helsinki declaration: all participants provided informed written consent prior to study enrolment.

Consent for publication

All participants and authors have approved the publication.

Competing interests

We have no competing interests to disclose.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

Semi-structured Interview Guide on Therapeutic Micro-processes in Inpatient ED Treatment.

Additional file 2.

Individual WAI Tasks subscale scores during treatment.

Additional file 3.

Individual WAI Goals subscale scores during treatment.

Additional file 4.

Individual WAI Emotional Bond subscale scores during treatment.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Olofsson, M.E., Vrabel, K.R., Hoffart, A. et al. Covert therapeutic micro-processes in non-recovered eating disorders with childhood trauma: an interpersonal process recall study. J Eat Disord 10, 42 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-022-00566-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-022-00566-1

Keywords