Skip to main content

Neural, physiological, and psychological markers of appetitive conditioning in anorexia nervosa: a study protocol

Abstract

Background

Anorexia nervosa (AN) is a chronic and disabling psychiatric condition characterized by low hedonic drive towards food, and is thought to be inclusive of altered dimensions of reward processing. Whether there exists a fundamental aberrancy in the capacity to acquire and maintain de novo hedonic associations—a critical component of hedonic responding—has never been studied in AN.

Methods

This multi-modal study will employ a 2-day Pavlovian appetitive conditioning paradigm to interrogate the (1) acquisition, (2) extinction, (3) spontaneous recovery and (4) reinstatement of appetitive learning in adolescents and young adults with AN. Participants will be 30 currently ill, underweight individuals with AN; 30 weight-restored individuals with AN; and 30 age-matched healthy controls, all aged 12–22 years. All subjects will undergo clinical assessment, followed by the 2-day appetitive conditioning task during which fMRI, pupillometry, heart rate deceleration, and subjective ratings will be acquired.

Discussion

This study will be the first to interrogate appetitive conditioning in AN—a disorder characterized by altered hedonic responding to food. Results will help establish objective biomarkers of appetitive conditioning in AN and lay the groundwork for developing novel lines of treatment for AN and other psychiatric disorders involving diminished ability to experience pleasure and reward.

Trial registration: Pending.

Intended registry

Clinicaltrials.gov.

Background

Anorexia nervosa (AN) is a disabling, often chronic, and potentially life-threatening eating disorder which is characterized by self-imposed starvation, emaciation, intense fear of weight gain, and a marked disturbance in how one’s body shape and weight is experienced [1]. AN demonstrates the greatest mortality rate of any psychiatric illness, with a crude mortality rate of 5.6% [2]. Even in nonlethal presentations, multi-systemic organ damage, bone disease, and structural and functional brain impairment are commonplace [3], which cumulatively render functional impairments comparable to those seen in schizophrenia [4] and autism [5]. Despite decades of investigation, the benefits of specialized treatments remain limited [6]. Remission rates typically range from 0 to 25% for adult presentations and approximately 15–33% for adolescent presentations [7], and more than half of those afflicted with AN still meet diagnostic criteria more than 2 decades after illness onset [8]. Importantly, the pathophysiology of AN is incompletely understood, stymying efforts towards improved, precision treatments. In short, treatment research has stalled, and the need to explicate the pathogenesis of AN cannot be overstated.

Owing to the centrality of food restriction, despite its near universal hedonic properties [9], much research investigating potential pathogenic mechanisms in AN has explored dimensions of reward-related processes. Cumulatively, research has pointed to aberrant experiences of reward; this includes the phenomenological observations, and self-reports, of diminished hedonic responses to typically hedonic cues [10, 11]. This is perhaps most evident in the context of food cues. Those with AN (1) rate olfactory, visual, and taste cues as markedly less pleasant than do healthy controls [12, 13]; (2) report increased negative affect after meals [14]; and (3) report food consumption to be aversive, rather than hedonic [15, 16]. Importantly, diminished hedonia such as in social situations is evident beyond the confines of food consumption among those with AN, with evidence suggesting a reduced drive to pursue novelty and fun [17, 18] and unease in social and sexual relationships [19, 20], which persists even after remission. This accords with the developmental history of those with AN, which is foreshadowed by reserve, caution, regimentation, and a lowered drive for pleasure and novelty seeking [10, 11, 21]. Thus, reduced responding to typically appetitive rewards, which, prima facie, could be the result of an atypical capacity to abstain from these rewards, may alternatively indicate a diminished capacity for hedonic responses. For either alternative, disturbance in appetitive responding is a potentially salient contributor to overall morbidity in terms of not only restricted food consumption, but impaired social interactions and overall diminished quality of life.

Alterations in hedonic responding are also reflected in psychophysiological markers. For instance, upon exposure to palatable food cues, those with AN demonstrate reduced zygomatic and greater corrugator facial muscle activity [22, 23], suggesting less positive and more negative affective states. Similarly, in social contexts, those with AN typically demonstrate avoidance of positive social cues [24, 25] and less positive facial affect and more negative facial affect to positive social cues [26, 27]. In concert, studies assessing dopaminergic function—a critical neurotransmitter relating to reward learning and hedonic responding—suggest trait level abnormalities in those with AN. For instance, reduced concentrations of CSF homovanillic acid—a dopamine metabolite—have been found in those with AN, even after recovery [28], and greater dopamine receptor availability has been noted in the ventral striatum of those with AN [29].

Underlying structural abnormalities in the brain’s reward circuitry have also been observed in AN, which extends to gray matter structures and white matter fiber tracts. Gray matter abnormalities are characterized by reduced volume in key nodes of the brain’s reward circuit, including the ventral striatum and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) [30, 31], and aberrant connectivity of white matter tracts between these regions [32, 33]. Moreover, preliminary evidence suggests that disturbed frontocortical-accumbal connectivity is associated with the severity of AN symptomatology [32].

Despite the consistency of these multimodal findings in suggesting disturbed hedonic responding in AN, findings from functional MRI studies are surprisingly varied. In the context of food cues, studies have generally noted reduced cortico-striatal activity among those with AN [34, 35], which extends to the taste, smell, and sight of palatable foods [34, 36,37,38]. In contrast, some studies of AN have illustrated hyperactivity in reward-related regions, such as the ventral striatum [39] and amygdala [40] in response to palatable-calorie food cues, despite those with AN implicitly and explicitly stating ‘liking’ and ‘wanting’ these foods less than controls [12]. At the same time, elevations in prefrontal regions including the medial prefrontal cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex following exposure to palatable food cues has driven several hypotheses suggesting elevated top-down inhibition of reward processing [41,42,43]. In keeping with behavioral data, aberrant neural response to hedonic cues extends beyond food cues. For instance, those with AN show abnormal cortico-striatal activity in response to monetary rewards [44], which accords with behavioral data suggesting a reduced proclivity towards valuing and pursuing immediate monetary gain [45].

Paradoxically, those with AN describe the restriction of food consumption, and feeling hungry, as intensely rewarding [10, 11]. This raises the intriguing possibility that dimensions of reward processing may remain intact but have nearly opposite associations as those in healthy individuals. That is, cues and states typically described as rewarding among those with AN (i.e., food restriction, hunger) are those typically described as aversive among those without AN [10, 11]. Contributing to this notion, the portrayal of self-starvation and relentless exercise as positive experiences and desirable lifestyle choices are themes that emerge from interviews of those with AN [46, 47]. This is consistent with data demonstrating elevated ventral striatum activity following exposure to images of thin people [48, 49] and elevated OFC activity observed in response to low calorie foods, which is exacerbated by fasting levels of acylated ghrelin [50]. This suggests that hunger may amplify the reward processing of low-calorie foods.

What could account for this subjective experience of the hunger state and low body weight as rewarding, however, remains unknown. It is possible that what is subjectively ‘rewarding’ is the avoidance of the perceived threat of food intake or normative body mass. Nevertheless, the mechanisms to explain the divergence of reward system responses are likely not simple, necessitating an understanding at higher levels of complexity, and across multiple units of analysis [51].

Whether there exists a fundamental aberrancy in hedonic processes in AN is unknown and has been minimally studied. Existing studies have exclusively interrogated reward circuitry in the circumscribed contexts of decision making, cognitive processes and prediction error around cues with an assumed incentive salience (i.e., money, sucrose) [52,53,54]. Yet, significant gaps remain in our understanding of key reward-related constructs of hedonic processing and appetitive behavioral learning, and no studies to date have interrogated the capacity for de novo learning of hedonic associations in AN. Ascertaining the integrity of appetitive learning systems is of critical importance in AN, for which an ongoing indifference to palatable food consumption often persists despite specialized treatments [55, 56] and may contribute to its high rates of relapse [57]. Illness remission across all treatment modalities is predicated on a therapeutic ability to enhance the incentive salience of food cues and promote approach behaviors and greater food consumption as well as help remediate impairments in social functioning [18]. As such, an increased understanding of appetitive learning in AN, and its decay and reinstatement, will directly inform efforts to augment patient outcomes by altering the incentive salience of recovery-congruent cues and food-related approach behaviors.

Appetitive Pavlovian conditioning paradigms offer a well-validated methodology to interrogate how positively-valenced associations are acquired, extinguished, and reinstated [58]. Perturbations have been linked to disease-specific symptomatology; appetitive conditioning studies in depression—a disorder highly comorbid and genetically correlated with AN [59]—illustrate abnormal activity in the amygdala, ventral striatum (VS), and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), which results in a failure to generate positive associations to typically hedonic cues [60]. Appetitive conditioning studies frequently employ palatable food cues (i.e., chocolate, sucrose) as the unconditioned stimulus (US). Importantly, however, the use of tastants in AN populations is problematic as it is conflated with the existing aversive associations to palatable food tastants. Disorder-neutral USs are thus warranted.

In addition, the potentially confounding effects of weight and nutritional status are important considerations. Animal studies suggest that food restriction and weight loss drive a sensitization of brain reward pathways [61,62,63] and dopaminergic neuronal activity [64]. This adaptive sensitization of D1 and D2/D3 receptors in response to food restriction is thought to stimulate motivation to approach food [65]. In keeping, the dopamine-mediated prediction error response is elevated in the NAcc, head of the caudate, and insula in underweight adolescents with AN [52], although this appears to normalize upon weight restoration [66]. In addition, some of the volumetric abnormalities in gray matter structures in the reward circuit are thought to normalize after weight restoration [67]. Current consensus in AN research advocates separating underweight from weight-restored patients with AN when unraveling the potential characteristics of the AN phenotype from state-specific effects of starvation [10].

With these considerations, we designed the present multimodal Pavlovian appetitive conditioning study, in underweight and weight-restored AN, to index the acquisition, extinction, spontaneous reinstatement, and recovery of associative reward learning—a key hypothesized mechanism in the pathogenesis and maintenance of AN. We will study these phenomena across multiple domains of analysis: subjective experience (self-report), physiology (heart rate deceleration and pupillary dilation), and neural response (brain activity in reward systems). Our conditioning paradigm employs positively-valenced, socially rewarding yet symptom-neutral (avoiding stimuli to which they may have been aversively conditioned) infant laughter sounds, which adolescents with AN typically rate positively (see Additional file 1). This study aims to assess in adolescent and transition age youth with restrictive type AN (AN-R) and healthy controls (1) the rate of association of a neutral cue with a paired hedonic outcome, (2) the rate of the decay of this association upon cessation of cue co-pairing, (3) the rate of spontaneous recovery (i.e., the increase in CS responding, after extinction, with the passage of time) and reinstatement (i.e., the increase in CS responding after extinction, following the presentation of the US alone) of this association upon cue exposure 24 h later, and (4) the patterns of physiological (heart rate deceleration, pupil dilation) and neural activation (fMRI) associated with acquisition, decay and reinstatement of this reward learning. In discerning whether any perturbations in reward learning represent state-specific effects of starvation or stable features of AN, this study will include underweight restricting-type AN (AN-R) patients, weight restored AN-R (WRAN-R) patients, and age-matched healthy controls. Further, with empirical findings noting stable sex differences in associative learning [68], the proposed study will include only female participants.

We hypothesize that all three groups will (1) demonstrate appetitive conditioning effects to the CS+ (the CS paired with the US) during the acquisition phase of the conditioning paradigm relative to the CS− (the CS not paired with the US), as evidenced by: positive experience and expectancy ratings, heart rate deceleration, pupillary dilation; and neural activation in the ventral striatum (node of the cortico-striatal circuit involved in appetitive conditioning) for the CS+ versus CS− contrast [58, 69]. Further, we predict that (2) across all groups, heart rate deceleration and pupillary dilation will moderate the relationship between ventral striatum activity and positive experience ratings. Further, we will ascertain whether (3) differences exist between AN groups (AN-R and WRAN-R) versus healthy controls in the rate of (a) acquisition, as well as (exploratory) (b) extinction, (c) spontaneous recovery, and (d) reinstatement of learned appetitive associations across neurological, physiological and self-report data. Moreover, and in delineating the effects of starvation among those with AN, we hypothesize that (4) those with AN-R, relative to those with WRAN-R, will demonstrate lower mean positive experience ratings, reduced heart rate deceleration, reduced pupillary dilation, and reduced neural activity for the CS+ − CS− contrast in the ventral striatum. We will also explore correlations between subjective experience ratings and both neural activity and physiological signals, within and across groups, to determine if AN (in the acute and/or weight restored state) is marked by a disconnect between subjective experience and neural and physiological phenomena.

Methods

Reproducibility

The trial protocol is openly available at (https://osf.io/nmgc2/). Moreover, this study will be prospectively registered with ClinicalTrials.gov prior to the collection of any data, and upon completion of data collection, summary data will be made publicly available. In addition, analytic scripts will be openly available at (https://osf.io/nmgc2/).

Participants

Participants will be female adolescents and transitional age youth (aged 12–22 years) with a DSM-5 diagnosis of AN-R who are (1) currently underweight (N = 30), (2) weight-restored (WRANR) (N = 30), and (3) age-matched healthy controls (N = 30). In delineating underweight vs. weight restored AN-R, a weight inclusion criterion of < 87% of age-, height-, and sex-adjusted expected weight will be upheld for the underweight AN group [70]. Control participants scoring higher than two standard deviations from community norms on measures of ED pathology (i.e., > 2 on the Global Eating Disorders Examination score [71]) will be excluded. To obtain a clinically representative sample, we will not exclude comorbid anxiety and depressive disorders. All participants must have the ability to read and speak English fluently, and anyone demonstrating (1) medical instability, (2) a change in dose of psychotropic medication over the previous 4 weeks, (3) anyone taking antipsychotic medications, or (4) any contraindications for MRI, will be excluded. Recruitment will take place in the Greater Toronto and Hamilton, Ontario Area (> 7.35 million), with multiple University-based and public and private eating disorder treatment programs. Community recruitment will include social media, online advertisements, and flyers.

fMRI appetitive conditioning paradigm

A 2-day differential Pavlovian conditioning paradigm widely used in human conditioning research will be used in this study. The unconditioned stimulus (US) will be infant laughter sounds, owing to the strong positive valence of infant laughter sounds [72], and our pilot data showing the positive self-reported valence of this sound among adolescents with AN (see Additional file 1). Prior to conditioning and owing to inter-individual variation in the degree to which each infant laughter cue is perceived as positive, participants will be exposed to a set of 18 baby laughter cues; the top 4 most positive rated cues for each individual will serve as the US in the conditioning paradigm. The conditioned stimuli (CSs) in this paradigm will be two distinctly colored geometric shapes. The US (the 4 most positively rated cues of infant laughter) will be delivered binaurally via noise canceling headphones (500 ms, 80dBA) and will co-terminate with the CS+. Notably, despite background MR scanner noise, infant laughter cues have been shown to elicit high magnitude BOLD signal changes in reward-related regions [73].

The conditioning paradigm consists of the following phases: Day 1—(1) habituation, (2) acquisition, (3) extinction phase; Day 2—(4) spontaneous recovery, (5) reinstatement, (6) reinstatement test (See Fig. 1). Day 2 will occur 24 h after Day 1. CS presentation duration will be 5.73 s, with inter-trial intervals with fixation jittered between 1.9 and 11.5 s (mean 5.7 s) (see Fig. 2). The jittering across all trial phases was optimized for detection efficiency, with an exponential distribution of null events, using optseq2 (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/optseq/). No more than three of the same CS will be presented sequentially. In habituation, two trials each of the CS+ and CS− will be presented, respectively. During acquisition, 20 presentations of the CS+ (75% paired with the US) and 20 presentations of the CS− (never paired with the US) will be presented (40 trials). During extinction, 20 exposures of each CS without the US will be presented (40 trials). Recovery will be assessed 24 h after the original appetitive conditioning procedure, and will consist of 20 exposures of each CS without the US (40 trials). Following this, the reinstatement paradigm will first consist of a 3-min exposure to a fixation cue, during which 4 uncued exposures to the US (infant laughter) will be delivered binaurally via noise canceling headphones (500 ms, 102 dBA). Subsequently, 20 exposures of each CS without the US will be presented (40 trials).

Fig. 1
figure 1

A An overview of the 2-day appetitive conditioning paradigm, the temporal sequencing of task phases, and the overall timing parameters of each phase. B represents an overview of cue sequencing for the acquisition phase, when the CS+ is paired with the US, and C represents an overview of cue sequencing for the habituation, extinction, recovery, and reinstatement phases, when the CS+ is presented without the US

Fig. 2
figure 2

Schedule of enrollment, interventions, and assessments. *Recommended content can be displayed using various schematic formats. See SPIRIT 2013 Explanation and Elaboration for examples from protocols. **List specific timepoints in this row

This appetitive conditioning paradigm did not incorporate ED-salient cues for several reasons. Importantly, appetitive conditioning paradigms are intended to assess the extent to which a previously benign cue can be associated with a positively experienced cue. The use of benign cues as the CS is especially important—such that no prior positive or negative association exists already, as this would contaminate the associative learning which the paradigm is intended to assess. If the cue used as the CS has existing associations, this may result in an assessment of recall or counter conditioning, rather than pure appetitive conditioning. ED-relevant cues may have likely already acquired valence or associations for those with AN, and so their use in an appetitive conditioning paradigm would corrupt the core theoretical tenets of conditioning paradigms. Moreover, this existing association to ED-relevant cues would likely not extend to control participants, which would therefore mean that the paradigm assesses different mechanisms in AN and control participants, respectively.

With regard to the US, this is required to be universally and comparably hedonic across both groups, so that the positive association can be transferred to the CS over repeated cue pairings. While the use of disorder-relevant stimuli as the US may have relevance for those with AN, it is unlikely that disorder- relevant cues deemed positive and hedonic by those with AN (i.e., thin images) would have comparable positive valence in those without AN. As such, this would not facilitate a meaningful between-group comparison of appetitive conditioning. As such, and in keeping with these important theoretical tenets, ED-salient cues will not be employed in the paradigm.

Outcome measures

A summary and the timing of administration of outcome measures is outlined in Table 1.

Table 1 An overview of the timing and administration of outcome measures

Primary outcome measures

Subjective ratings A 11-point Likert scale (− 5 to 5) will assess self-reported pleasantness and arousal for each CS, and US expectancy [74, 75]. Ratings will be measured at the end of every phase.

Heart rate deceleration Heart rate deceleration has been strongly associated with appetitive Pavlovian conditioning (ηp2 of 0.8) [76]. Using a BioPac MP160 (Biopac, Goleta CA), we will measure heart rate deceleration/acceleration via heart period responses from each inter-trial interval to CS termination in both acquisition and extinction phases, and during the recovery and reinstatement phase. We will use a QRS detection algorithm and preprocess using PsychoPhysiological Modelling toolbox in Matlab (http://pspm.sourceforge.net/).

Pupillometry Increases in pupillary diameter have been associated with appetitive Pavlovian conditioning (ηp2 of 0.29) [76], and with locus coeruleus activity [77, 78], which itself is associated with orienting of attention and reward anticipation [79]. Further, pupillary dilation is not correlated with HR deceleration [76], providing a proxy for central neurophysiology that is distinct from heart period responses. We will track each participants’ pupillary diameter and eye movements using a high precision MR-compatible EyeLink 1000 Plus (SR Research, Ottawa, Ontario Canada) eye tracker camera that can sample up to 2000 Hz. We will smooth the data using a median filter. Pupillary diameter data will be baseline corrected using the mean diameter in a time window of 2 s prior to CS onset. We will convolve the stimulus onsets with a canonical pupillary response function [80].

fMRI acquisition and processing We will use a GE Discovery 3T scanner with a 32-channel head coil. T1-weighted MPRAGE (0.8 mm3) using HCP Lifespan protocols (humanconnectome.org) will be used for registration and to measure gray matter volumes and thickness for exploratory covariates, as they can influence regional activation measures. Task functional data will be acquired using the HCP Lifespan multiband sequence: 2 mm3 and TR = 955 ms (https://www.cmrr.umn.edu/multiband), and spin echo field maps to correct for geometric distortions. Image Processing will be done with fMRIPrep [81]. Spatial normalization of the T1 image to standard MNI space will be performed through nonlinear registration. The BOLD timeseries with slice-timing correction will be resampled onto their native space by applying a single, composite transform to correct for head-motion and (estimated) susceptibility distortions. The BOLD timeseries will then be resampled into standard MNI space, generating the spatially-normalized, preprocessed BOLD runs. Automatic removal of motion artifacts using independent component analysis (ICA-AROMA) will be performed after spatial smoothing with a Gaussian kernel of 6 mm FWHM. We will use reward ROIs involved in appetitive conditioning [58] to test our hypotheses involving the nucleus accumbens (NAcc), and exploratory ROIs in the appetitive conditioning circuit [58] including the OFC, amygdala, ventral tegmental area (VTA), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC). Probabilistic ROIs will be obtained from the following atlases: OFC, ACC, PCC (Harvard–Oxford atlas); central and basolateral amygdala (Juelich Histological Atlas); NAcc, VTA (Reinforcement Learning Atlas [82]). Eigenvalues from the first eigenvariate of voxels within each ROI over the timeseries for the CS+ versus CS− contrast will be derived, indexing brain activation.

Baseline measures

Medical history All AN participants will be screened for medical stability, and any participant with acute medical instability will be excluded. In addition, participant height and weight will be measured for all participants, and all participants will be screened for general contraindications for MRI scanning procedures.

Eating disorder examination [71] The Eating Disorder Examination (EDE) is a widely-used clinician-administered diagnostic interview with excellent psychometric properties [71]. This interview assesses eating symptomatology over the preceding 28 days, and comprises four subscales; Dietary Restraint, Eating Concern, Shape Concern and Weight Concern. The EDE will be administered by licensed and EDE-trained mental health professionals with eating disorder expertise.

MINI/MINI-KID diagnostic interview [83] The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI 7.0.2) and Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview for Children and Adolescents (MINI-KID) are robust and psychometrically strong structured diagnostic interviews for adults and children, respectively, for DSM and ICD psychiatric disorders [83]. The MINI and MINI-KID will be administered by licensed mental health professionals.

Behavioral Inhibition Scale/Behavioral Approach Scale [84] The Behavioral Inhibition Scale /Behavioral Approach Scale (BIS/BAS) is a measure of individual sensitivities in the drive to avoid potentially aversive outcomes and approach goal-oriented outcomes, showing good psychometric properties [84].

Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale [85] The Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS) is a widely used dimensional measure of anxiety, depression, and stress, which demonstrates strong psychometric properties and high internal consistency [86].

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory [87] The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) is a self-report measure of state and trait anxiety, which has been widely used in both eating disorder [88] and conditioning research [89], and demonstrates excellent psychometric properties [87]. For a comprehensive overview of trial protocol and study measures, please see Additional file: 2 for a Spirit checklist.

Data analysis

Power analysis

The sample is powered for the primary outcomes for our primary hypotheses based on an alpha of 0.0125 (corrected for 4 independent tests for each dependent variable) and accounting for the primary four phases of conditioning. For the whole-sample hypotheses, N = 90 total provides > 95% power to detect previously reported large effect sizes for heart rate and pupil dilation (ηp2 = 0.8 and 0.29, respectively) [76], and NAcc CS+ versus CS− contrasts [69]. While effect sizes for appetitive conditioning (1) with infant laughter, and (2) in AN, are unknown, we have 86% power to detect a small effect size (ηp2 = 0.042). If recruitment and enrollment goals are not met, for our primary hypothesis a total sample size of N = 72 (n = 24 in each of 3 groups) would be able to detect a medium effect size of ηp2 = 0.06 with 89% power, while N = 60 (n = 20 in each of 3 groups) would be able to detect a medium effect size of ηp2 = 0.06 with 80% power. For our hypotheses comparing AN-R and WRANR, a sample size of 60 provides > 95% power to detect similar large effect sizes for heart rate and pupillary dilation (ηp2 = 0.29 to 0.8), 90% power to detect a medium effect size (ηp2 = 0.06), and 72% power to detect a small effect size (ηp2 = 0.042).

Analysis plan

Variable transformations prior to subsequent analyses will be determined based on distribution diagnostics and outlier analysis. Hypotheses (1), (3), and (4) will be tested using general linear models with between-subject factor of group (AN, WRAN; control), within-subject factors of stimulus (CS+ vs. CS−) and time (post-acquisition vs. post-extinction vs. spontaneous recovery vs. reinstatement), and their interactions. Covariates include age, age2, pubertal development scores, medication status, and depression symptom severity. These will be done separately for each of the dependent variables (Bonferroni-corrected): self-reported positive ratings, self-reported expectancy ratings, heart period response (specifically, operationalized primary deceleration [90]), pupillary response, and mean BOLD eigenvariate magnitude in the NAcc for the CS+ versus CS− contrast [69]. Exploratory analyses will be done with other appetitive conditioning ROIs (OFC, ACC, PCC, amygdala, and VTA), as well as for self-reported rating for pleasantness, arousal, and reward expectancy. To test hypothesis (2), we will use PROCESS v3.5 operated in SPSS, employing bootstrapping with 5000 resamples [91] to (separately) model moderation effects of (a) heart period response (primary deceleration) and (b) pupillary response on the relationship between NAcc eigenvariate magnitude for the CS+ versus CS− contrast (independent variable) and positive experience ratings (dependent variable).

Discussion

The study proposed here will be the first systematic interrogation of appetitive conditioning in AN. Successful long-term treatment for AN hinges not only on a therapeutic ability to optimize approach behaviors to food consumption to halt the proximal, life-threatening symptoms related to starvation, but also to remediate the ability to respond hedonically to social and other stimuli that may be required to motivate one to recover from an otherwise chronic illness and stay in a remitted state. Without the help of the proverbial “carrot” of life enjoyment, the fear of the “stick” (weight gain, perceived fatness, and imperfection) will remain the dominant motivator of behaviors.

Existing studies have cast a varied picture of diminished or elevated reward responses in AN, depending on the stimulus, nutritional state, and likely other latent factors. Yet, critically, whether individuals can learn to respond to new rewarding stimuli has not been studied. This study will examine whether underweight or weight-restored individuals with AN can be conditioned to learn positive responses to non-symptom-related rewarding stimuli, as measured by subjective experience, physiology, and brain responses. Non-symptom-related cues—in this case, the socially rewarding sounds of infant laughter—allow us to examine the functional integrity of mechanisms underpinning appetitive learning. Further, these cues will mitigate the potentially confounding effects of learned associations that are linked to symptoms of AN.

Data obtained from the study will be used to develop a model (to be tested and validated in future data sets) to predict appetitive conditioning from physiological and neural variables. An additional next step would be to determine if symptom-related, potentially hedonic cues can be counter-conditioned—that is, if those previously aversively conditioned such as food can acquire positive valence. Results will help establish objective biomarkers of appetitive conditioning in AN and lay the groundwork for developing novel lines of treatment for AN and other psychiatric disorders involving diminished ability to experience pleasure and reward.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and analyzed during the current study contain clinical data and are not publicly available due to the protection of participants’ rights to privacy and data protection, but are available from SBM and JDF upon reasonable request. Summary data will be made available on ClinicalTrials.gov.

Abbreviations

AN:

Anorexia nervosa

CS:

Conditioned stimulus

US:

Unconditioned stimulus

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  2. Arcelus J, Mitchell AJ, Wales J, Nielsen S. Mortality rates in patients with anorexia nervosa and other eating disorders: a meta-analysis of 36 studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68:724–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Mitchell JE, Crow S. Medical complications of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2006;19:438–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Vos T, Mathers CD. The burden of mental disorders: a comparison of methods between the Australian burden of disease studies and the global burden of disease study. Bull World Health Organ. 2000;78:427–38.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Whiteford HA, Ferrari AJ, Degenhardt L, Feigin V, Vos T. The global burden of mental, neurological and substance use disorders: an analysis from the global burden of disease study. PLoS ONE. 2010;10:e0116820.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Murray SB, Quintana DS, Loeb KL, Griffiths S, Le Grange D. Treatment outcomes for anorexia nervosa: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Psychol Med. 2019;49:535–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Watson HJ, Bulik CM. Update on the treatment of anorexia nervosa: review of clinical trials, practice guidelines and emerging interventions. Psychol Med. 2013;43:2477–500.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Fichter MM, Quadflieg N, Crosby RD, Koch S. Long-term outcome of anorexia nervosa: results from a large clinical longitudinal study. Int J Eat Disord. 2017;50:1018–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Kringelbach ML. Food for thought: hedonic experience beyond homeostasis in the human brain. Neuroscience. 2004;126:807–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Kaye WH, Fudge JL, Paulus M. New insights into symptoms and neurocircuit function of anorexia nervosa. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:573–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Kaye WH, Wierenga CE, Bailer UF, Simmons AN, Bischoff-Grethe A. Nothing tastes as good as skinny feels: the neurobiology of anorexia nervosa. Trends Neurosci. 2013;36:110–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Cowdrey FA, Finlayson G, Park RJ. Liking compared with wanting for high- and low-calorie foods in anorexia nervosa: aberrant food reward even after weight restoration. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;97:463–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Jiang T, Soussignan R, Rigaud D, Schaal B. Pleasure for visual and olfactory stimuli evoking energy-dense foods is decreased in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 2010;180:42–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Anderson LM, Crow SJ, Peterson CB. The impact of meal consumption on emotion among individuals with eating disorders. Eat Weight Disord Stud Anorex Bulim Obes. 2013;19:347–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Bruch H. The golden cage: the enigma of anorexia nervosa. Harvard University Press; 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Bruch H. Conversations with anorexics: a compassionate and hopeful journey through the therapeutic process. Oxford: Basic Books; 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Boehm I, Flohr L, Steding J, Holzapfel L, Seitz J, Roessner V, et al. The trajectory of anhedonic and depressive symptoms in anorexia nervosa: a longitudinal and cross-sectional approach. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2018;26:69–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Harrison A, Mountford VA, Tchanturia K. Social anhedonia and work and social functioning in the acute and recovered phases of eating disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2014;218:187–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Pinheiro AP, Raney TJ, Thornton LM, Fichter MM, Berrettini WH, Goldman D, et al. Sexual functioning in women with eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord. 2009;43:123–9.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Gonidakis F, Kravvariti V, Varsou E. Sexual function of women suffering from anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. J Sex Marital Ther. 2015;41:368–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Anderluh MB, Tchanturia K, Rabe-Hesketh S, Treasure J. Childhood obsessive-compulsive personality traits in adult women with eating disorders: defining a broader eating disorder phenotype. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:242–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Soussignan R, Jiang T, Rigaud D, Royet JP, Schaal B. Subliminal fear priming potentiates negative facial reactions to food pictures in women with anorexia nervosa. Psychol Med. 2010;40:503–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Soussignan R, Schaal B, Rigaud D, Royet JP, Jiang T. Hedonic reactivity to visual and olfactory cues: rapid facial electromyographic reactions are altered in anorexia nervosa. Biol Psychol. 2011;86:265–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Cardi V, Di Matteo R, Corfield F, Treasure J. Social reward and rejection sensitivity in eating disorders: an investigation of attentional bias and early experiences. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2013;14:622–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Watson KK, Werling DM, Zucker NL, Platt ML. Altered social reward and attention in anorexia nervosa. Front Psychol. 2010;1:36.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Cardi V, Corfield F, Leppanen J, Rhind C, Deriziotis S, Hadjimichalis A, et al. Emotional processing, recognition, empathy and evoked facial expression in eating disorders: an experimental study to map deficits in social cognition. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0133827.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Davies H, Schmidt U, Tchanturia K. Emotional facial expression in women recovered from anorexia nervosa. BMC Psychiatry. 2013;13:291.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Kaye WH, Frank GK, McConaha C. Altered dopamine activity after recovery from restricting-type anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999;21:503–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Frank GK, Bailer UF, Henry SE, Drevets W, Meltzer CC, Price JC, et al. Increased dopamine D2/D3 receptor binding after recovery from anorexia nervosa measured by positron emission tomography and [11c]raclopride. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58:908–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Frank GK, Shott ME, Hagman JO, Mittal VA. Alterations in brain structures related to taste reward circuitry in ill and recovered anorexia nervosa and in bulimia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170:1152–60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Titova OE, Hjorth OC, Schiöth HB, Brooks SJ. Anorexia nervosa is linked to reduced brain structure in reward and somatosensory regions: a meta-analysis of VBM studies. BMC Psychiatry. 2013;13:110.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Cha J, Ide JS, Bowman FD, Simpson HB, Posner J, Steinglass JE. Abnormal reward circuitry in anorexia nervosa: a longitudinal, multimodal MRI study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016;37:3835–46.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Via E, Zalesky A, Sánchez I, Forcano L, Harrison BJ, Pujol J, et al. Disruption of brain white matter microstructure in women with anorexia nervosa. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2014;39:367–75.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Jiang T, Soussignan R, Carrier E, Royet JP. Dysfunction of the mesolimbic circuit to food odors in women with anorexia and bulimia nervosa: a fMRI Study. Front Hum Neurosci. 2019;13:117.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Brooks SJ, O’Daly OG, Uher R, Friederich HC, Giampietro V, Brammer M, et al. Differential neural responses to food images in women with bulimia versus anorexia nervosa. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e22259.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Holsen L, Lawson EA, Blum J, Ko E, Makris N, Fazeli PK, et al. Food motivation circuitry hypoactivation related to hedonic and nonhedonic aspects of hunger and satiety in women with active anorexia nervosa and weight-restored women with anorexia nervosa. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2012;37:322–32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. McFadden KL, Tregellas JR, Shott ME, Frank GKW. Reduced salience and default mode network activity in women with anorexia nervosa. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2014;39:178–88.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Oberndorfer TA, Frank GKW, Simmons AN, Wagner A, McCurdy D, Fudge JL, et al. Altered insula response to sweet taste processing after recovery from anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170:1143–51.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Cowdrey FA, Park RJ, Harmer CJ, McCabe C. Increased neural processing of rewarding and aversive food stimuli in recovered anorexia nervosa. Biol Psychiat. 2011;70:736–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Vocks S, Busch M, Grönemeyer D, Schulte D, Herpertz S, Suchan B. Neural correlates of viewing photographs of one’s own body and another woman’s body in anorexia and bulimia nervosa: an fMRI study. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2010;35:163–76.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Kerr KL, Moseman SE, Avery JA, Bodurka J, Simmons WK. Influence of visceral interoceptive experience on the brain’s response to food images in anorexia nervosa. Psychosom Med. 2017;79:777–84.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Uher R, Brammer MJ, Murphy T, Campbell IC, Ng VW, Williams SCR, et al. Recovery and chronicity in anorexia nervosa: brain activity associated with differential outcomes. Biol Psychiatty. 2003;54:934–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Uher R, Murphy T, Brammer MJ, Dalgleish T, Phillips ML, Ng VW, et al. Medial prefrontal cortex activity associated with symptom provocation in eating disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:1238–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Wagner A, Aizenstein H, Venkatraman VK, Fudge J, May JC, Mazurkewicz L, et al. Altered reward processing in women recovered from anorexia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:1842–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Steinglass JE, Lempert KM, Choo T-H, Kimeldorf MB, Wall M, Walsh BT, et al. Temporal discounting across three psychiatric disorders: anorexia nervosa, obsessive compulsive disorder, and social anxiety disorder. Depress Anxiety. 2017;34:463–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Serpell L, Treasure J, Teasdale J, Sullivan V. Anorexia nervosa: friend or foe? Int J Eat Disord. 1999;25:177–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Brotsky SR, Giles D. Inside the “pro-ana” community: a covert online participant observation. Eat Disord. 2007;15:93–109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Fladung A-K, Grön G, Grammer K, Gerrnberger B, Schilly E, Grasteit S, et al. A neural signature of anorexia nervosa in the ventral striatal reward system. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167:206–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Fladung A-K, Schulze UME, Schöll F, Bauer K, Grön G. Role of the ventral striatum in developing anorexia nervosa. Transl Psychiatry. 2013;3:e315.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Holsen LM, Lawson EA, Christensen K, Klibanski A, Goldstein JM. Abnormal relationships between the neural response to high- and low-calorie foods and endogenous acylated ghrelin in women with active and weight-recovered anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 2014;223:94–103.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Haynos AF, Lavender JM, Nelson J, Crow SJ, Peterson CB. Moving towards specificity: a systematic review of cue features associated with reward and punishment in anorexia nervosa. Clin Psychol Rev. 2020;79:101872.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Frank GKW, DeGuzman MC, Shott ME, Laudenslager ML, Rossi B, Pryor T. Association of brain reward learning response with harm avoidance, weight gain, and hypothalamic effective connectivity in adolescent anorexia nervosa. JAMA Psychiat. 2018;75:1071–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Monteleone AM, Monteleone P, Esposito F, Prinster A, Volpe U, Canton E, et al. Altered processing of rewarding and aversive basic taste stimuli in symptomatic women with anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: an fMRI study. J Psychiatry Res. 2017;90:94–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Steding J, Boehm I, King JA, Geisler D, Ritschel F, Seidel M, et al. Goal-directed vs. habitual instrumental behavior during reward processing in anorexia nervosa: an fMRI study. Sci Rep. 2019;9:13529.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Pike KM. Long-term course of anorexia nervosa: response, relapse, remission, and recovery. Clin Psychol Rev. 1998;18:447–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Schebendach JE, Mayer LE, Devlin MJ, Attia E, Contento IR, Wolf RL, et al. Dietary energy density and diet variety as predictors of outcome in anorexia nervosa. Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;87:810–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Carter JC, Blackmore E, Sutandar-Pinnock K, Woodside B. Relapse in anorexia nervosa: a survival analysis. Psychol Med. 2004;4:671–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Martin-Soelch C, Linthicum J, Ernst M. Appetitive conditioning: neural bases and implications for psychopathology. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2007;31:426–40.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Watson HJ, Yilmaz Z, Thornton LM, Hübel C, Coleman JRI, Gaspar HA, et al. Genome-wide association study identifies eight risk loci and implicates metabo-psychiatric origins for anorexia nervosa. Nat Genet. 2019;51:1207–14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Drevets WC. Neuroimaging and neuropathological studies of depression: implications for the cognitive-emotional features of mood disorders. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2001;11:240–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Carr KD. Chronic food restriction: enhancing effects on drug reward and striatal cell signaling. Physiol Behav. 2007;91:459–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Carr KD, Tsimberg Y, Berman Y, Yamamoto N. Evidence of increased dopamine receptor signaling in food-restricted rats. Neuroscience. 2003;119:1157–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Avena NM, Rada P, Hoebel BG. Underweight rats have enhanced dopamine release and blunted acetylcholine response in the nucleus accumbens while bingeing on sucrose. Neuroscience. 2008;156:865–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Oinio V, Bäckström P, Uhari-Väänänen J, Raasmaja A, Piepponen P, Kiianmaa K, et al. Dopaminergic modulation of reward-guided decision making in alcohol-preferring AA rats. Behav Brain Res. 2017;326:87–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Frank GKW, DeGuzman MC, Shott ME. Motivation to eat and not to eat—the psychobiological conflict in anorexia nervosa. Physiol Behav. 2019;206:185–90.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. DeGuzman M, Shott ME, Yang TT, Riederer J, Frank GKW. Association of elevated reward prediction error response with weight gain in adolescent anorexia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry. 2017;174:557–65.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Bernardoni F, King JA, Geisler D, Stein E, Jaite C, Nätsch D, et al. Weight restoration therapy rapidly reverses cortical thinning in anorexia nervosa: a longitudinal study. Neuroimage. 2016;130:214–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Dalla C, Shors TJ. Sex differences in learning processes of classical and operant conditioning. Physiol Behav. 2009;97:229–38.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Tapia León I, Kruse O, Stalder T, Stark R, Klucken T. Neural correlates of subjective CS/UCS association in appetitive conditioning. Hum Brain Mapp. 2018;39:1637–46.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Lock J, Le Grange D, Agras WS, Fitzpatrick KK, Jo B, Accurso E, et al. Can adaptive treatment improve outcomes in family-based therapy for adolescents with anorexia nervosa? feasibility and treatment effects of a multi-site treatment study. Behav Res Ther. 2015;73:90–5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Fairburn CG, Cooper Z. The eating disorder examination. In: Fairburn CG, Wilson GT, editors. Binge eating: nature, assessment & treatment, vol. 12. New York: Guilford Press; 1993. p. 317–60.

    Google Scholar 

  72. Parsons CE, Young KS, Craske MG, Stein AL, Kringelbach ML. Introducing the oxford vocal (OxVoc) sounds database: a validated set of non-acted affective sounds from human infants, adults, and domestic animals. Front Psychol. 2014;5:562.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Riem MME, Ijzendoorn MHV, Tops M, Boksem MAS, Rombouts SARB, Kranenburg-Bakermans MJ. No laughing matter: intranasal oxytocin administration changes functional brain connectivity during exposure to infant laughter. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:1257–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Brown LA, LeBeau RT, Chat KY, Craske MG. Associative learning versus fear habituation as predictors of long-term extinction retention. Cogn Emot. 2017;31:687–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Staples-Bradley LK, Treanor M, Craske MG. Discrimination between safe and unsafe stimuli mediates the relationship between trait anxiety and return of fear. Cogn Emot. 2018;32:167–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Pietrock C, Ebrahimi C, Katthagen TM, Koch SP, Heinz A, Rothkirch M, et al. Pupil dilation as an implicit measure of appetitive Pavlovian learning. Psychophysiology. 2019;56:e13463.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Murphy PR, O’Connell RG, O’Sullivan M, Robertson IH, Balsters JH. Pupil diameter covaries with BOLD activity in human locus coeruleus. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35:4140–54.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Joshi S, Li Y, Kalwani RM, Gold JI. Relationships between pupil diameter and neuronal activity in the locus coeruleus, colliculi, and cingulate cortex. Neuron. 2016;89:221–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Bouret S, Sara SJ. Reward expectation, orientation of attention and locus coeruleus-medial frontal cortex interplay during learning. Eur J Neurosci. 2004;20:791–802.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Hoeks B, Levelt WJM. Pupillary dilation as a measure of attention: a quantitative system analysis. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 1993;25:16–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Esteban O, Markiewicz CJ, Blair RW, Moodie CA, Isik AI, Erramuzpe A, et al. fMRIPrep: a robust preprocessing pipeline for functional MRI. Nat Methods. 2019;16:111–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Pauli WM, Nili AN, Tyszka JM. A high-resolution probabilistic in vivo atlas of human subcortical brain nuclei. Sci Data. 2018;5:180063.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Sheehan DV, Sheehan KH, Shytle RD, Janavs J, Bannon Y, Rogers JE, et al. Reliability and validity of the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview for children and adolescents (MINI-KID). J Clin Psychiatry. 2010;71:313–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Carver CS, White TL. Behavioural inhibition, behavioural activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the BIS/BAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;67:319–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  85. Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH. The structure of the negative emotional states: comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behav Res Ther. 1995;33:335–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Mahmoud JSR, Hall LA, Staten R. The psychometric properties of the 21-item Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS-21) among a sample of young adults. South Online J Nurs Res. 2010;10:21–34.

    Google Scholar 

  87. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE. STAI manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1970.

    Google Scholar 

  88. Kaye WH, Bulik CM, Thornton L, Barbarich N, Masters K. Comorbidity of anxiety disorders with anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:2215–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Grillon C, Ameli R, Foot M, Davis M. Fear-potentiated startle: relationship to the level of state/trait anxiety in healthy subjects. Biol Psychiatry. 1993;33:566–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Paulus PC, Castegnetti G, Bach DR. Modeling event-related heart period responses. Psychophysiology. 2016;53:837–46.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  91. Hayes AF. Partial, conditional, and moderated moderated mediation: Quantification, inference, and interpretation. Commun Monogr. 2018;85:4–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Study stage: Data collection has not started.

Funding

Funding for this study is provided by the Klarman Family Foundation, a private foundation where grants are competitively reviewed and funded. The funding source did not play a role in designing the study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

S.B.M. and J.D.F. led the manuscript development, and will coordinate the study, and are responsible for its methodological design. In addition, S.B.M. and J.D.F. will oversee all diagnostic assessments. S.B.M., J.D.F., T.D.Z. and M.C. developed and pilot tested the appetitive conditioning paradigm. S.B.M., J.D.F., R.T., A.A.B., and J.O.D. developed the neuroimaging analysis plan. M.S. provided critical feedback on the methodological design of the study. All authors read, edited and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stuart B. Murray.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethics approval for this project was granted by the Institutional Review Boards at the University of Toronto; the University of California, Los Angeles; and at the University of Southern California. JDF and the research assistants at the University of Toronto will obtain informed consent and assent from potential trial participants. All participants in the study must sign a written consent form, or a written assent form, alongside a signed parent/guardian consent form, prior to participation.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1:

Pilot data.

Additional file 2:

Spirit checklist.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Murray, S.B., Zbozinek, T.D., Craske, M. et al. Neural, physiological, and psychological markers of appetitive conditioning in anorexia nervosa: a study protocol. J Eat Disord 10, 68 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-022-00546-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-022-00546-5

Keywords