Skip to main content

Food related attention bias modification training for anorexia nervosa and its potential underpinning mechanisms

Abstract

Treatment outcomes in anorexia nervosa (AN) remain suboptimal, evidencing the need for better and more targeted treatments. Whilst the aetiology of AN is complex, cognitive processes such as attention bias (AB) have been proposed to contribute to maintaining food restriction behaviour. Attention bias modification raining (ABMT) has been investigated in other eating disorders (EDs) such as binge eating disorder (BED) as a means of modifying AB for food and of changing eating behaviour. Promising findings have been reported, but the mechanisms underlying ABMT are poorly understood. We hypothesise that in AN, ABMT has the potential to modify maladaptive eating behaviours related to anxiety around food and eating and propose two mechanistic models; (1) ABMT increases general attentional control (which will improve control over disorder-relevant thoughts) or (2) ABMT promotes stimulus re-evaluation. In this second case, the effects of ABMT might arise via changes in the subjective value of food stimuli (i.e. reward processing) or via habituation, with both resulting in a reduced threat response. Investigating the clinical potential of ABMT in AN holds the promise of a novel, evidence-based adjunctive treatment approach. Importantly, understanding ABMT’s underlying mechanisms will help tailor treatment protocols and improve understanding of the cognitive characteristics of AN and other EDs.

Background

The involvement of cognitive mechanisms related to food processing, including food –related attention bias (AB) in the aetiology and maintenance of anorexia nervosa (AN) is well documented [1,2,3]. We propose that attention bias modification training (ABMT), a novel treatment approach, holds clinical potential for AN by modifying maladaptive AB for food. We also suggest that increasing knowledge of the neural/psychological processes involved in altering AB, will help maximize its clinical efficacy.

Attention bias to food in AN

AB is a cognitive process whereby salient stimuli (e.g. food), selectively “capture” attention in comparison to neutral cues [4]. This can occur outside of conscious control and it is thought to influence subsequent behaviour, such as food consumption [5]. Cognitive models suggest that people with AN show aberrant attention processing of food cues (i.e. AB) because of their preoccupation with and/or fear of food. In support of this, a meta-analysis found AB to food pictures with a medium effect size in a mixed sample of eating disorder (ED) patients [1]. Eye-tracking data showed that while initial AB to food cues in people with AN was comparable to healthy controls, those with AN did not maintain attention on the food cues [2, 3]. Rather, they present attentional avoidance [2, 3]. This attentional avoidance pattern was more evident when they were presented with high caloric food cues, in contrast to low caloric food cues [3]. As research in healthy individuals indicates that duration of attentional gaze on food is related to subsequent craving and to food intake [6], it has been proposed that attentional avoidance of, rather than increased initial engagement with high caloric food cues, could be a strategy used to reinforce food restriction [7]. Attentional avoidance of high caloric food cues is mainly seen in adults with AN (longer illness duration) compared to adolescents (shorter illness duration) [3], suggesting that this behaviour becomes more ingrained as illness progresses and may be an important element in maintaining fear of food or restrictive eating behaviour.

Attention bias modification training (ABMT)

ABMT is a form of cognitive bias modification training [8] with the potential for modifying AB triggered by different stimuli (e.g. food) [9]. Typically, ABMT is used to train attention towards disorder-incompatible stimuli in a relatively implicit way: it was developed by modifying the dot-probe paradigm used to assess AB [10]. In the original format of the task, participants are simultaneously presented with a disorder-relevant stimulus and a neutral stimulus on either side of a computer screen. Immediately after, a probe replaces one of the images and participants indicate the location of the probe as quickly as possible. AB is assumed when individuals respond faster when the probe replaces disorder- relevant stimuli. In the training version of the paradigm, the probe almost always (e.g. 95% of the time) replaces either the disorder-relevant or neutral stimulus, depending on the design. For example, if the aim of the training is to shift attention towards disorder-relevant cues, which would be the case for people with AN, the probe would be set to appear on the location of the threatening cue (e.g. high caloric food), manipulating attentional focus towards it, thereby reducing attentional avoidance.

While ABMT has been mainly used in the treatment of depression and anxiety disorders [11], a version involving training with food cues has been used in obesity and binge eating disorder (BED). Based on the hypothesis that sustained AB to food is associated with increased intake, the aim of ABMT in such studies was to reduce attention to food [7, 12]. In support of this, reviews and meta-analyses have described significant effects of different types of cognitive bias modification training (including ABMT) in changing AB and eating behaviour [13, 14].

Theoretical models underpinning ABMT

Despite encouraging results on the potential therapeutic value of AMBT in problematic eating behaviour and different psychiatric disorders, findings are inconsistent. This may be because of limited understanding of the mechanisms underpinning ABMT’s potential therapeutic effect, which, in turn, may lead to heterogeneous and untargeted training designs that lack solid theoretical underpinnings. To maximise the potential of ABMT, it is necessary to establish the underlying mechanisms, and tailor the training according to differences between target populations. Some studies suggest ABMT’s effects are substantially due to increased general attentional control [11]. However, others suggest that its effects are mediated primarily by changes in stimulus evaluation [15]. These mechanisms are discussed below.

Attention control model

This proposes that an increase in general attention control improves control of disorder-relevant thoughts, i.e., strengthening higher order cortical processes will down-regulate emotion-relevant limbic structures. Thus, in AN, enhancing executive functioning will improve emotional control, and this is proposed to lead to a reduction in rumination and fear of food, which are part of ED psychopathology (Fig. 1a). In support of this, some studies in anxiety disorders indicate that training attention (regardless of contingency between cues and probes), improves attention control, and this improves participants’ control over anxiety-related cognitions and emotions [11]. However, others suggest that attention control might not be the only, or even the primary mechanism involved. Thus, Taylor et al. [16] showed that a reduction in both AB and anxiety symptoms only occurred in participants undertaking the “active” ABMT condition in comparison to a non-contingency training. Furthermore, a neuroimaging study reported decreased neural activation in areas implicated in threat-response after training people to look away from threat cues, compared to before ABMT [17], evidencing the involvement of stimulus-specific attention processes. These studies [16, 17] suggest that ABMT acts via modulation of circuits linked to attention control, specifically to executive and affective control (so called “top-down” processes), and via changes to stimulus-driven or “bottom-up” mechanisms to modify AB. The potential role of “bottom-up” mechanisms, would be consistent with a model which proposes that ABMT changes stimulus evaluation. This is discussed below.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Theoretical models underpinning attention bias modification training (ABMT) for the treatment of anorexia nervosa (AN)

Re-evaluation model

Here, ABMT is proposed to change the way the stimulus is perceived and/or evaluated. Two potential (not exclusive) mechanisms may be involved: (a) re-evaluation of stimuli, due to repeated approach or avoidance behaviour, changes the rewarding properties of the stimulus (repeatedly approaching high-calorie food, makes them more rewarding for people with AN), or, (b) habituation, associated with repeated exposure, changes the valence of the stimulus (repeatedly looking at high-calorie food, makes them less threatening).

In support of this, Goetz et al. [18] investigated whether biasing the attention of healthy controls (HCs) towards or away from rewarding words (e.g. rewarding foods) would implicitly prime the appetitive system. They reported that manipulating attention increased self-reported approach motivation and food intake. In a similar way, the subjective value of foods can be modified via cue-approach behaviour (go/no go training) without external reinforcement or any other explicit manipulation of value [15]: Pairing certain cues with a “go” response will increase the value of these items as a result of training a motor approach response toward them. Hence, these and other studies suggest that training attention towards or away from food stimuli changes eating behaviour by altering the subjective value of food. If so, patients with AN would re-evaluate food cues more positively by repeatedly having to approach food with their eye-gaze. This change in approach behaviour should counter the attentional avoidance of food cues in people with AN, and in time, might change food intake (Fig. 1b).

An alternative explanation for a re-evaluative effect associated with ABMT may be related to repeated exposure to disorder-relevant cues. Exposure based on habituation models assumes a disruption of the fear avoidance reaction by learning that the feared consequences will not occur, leading to fear reduction and formation of new associations with the stimuli [19]. The premise is that ABMT is effective due to habituation and to a reduced fear response towards relevant stimuli (i.e. food), hence, food cues become less likely to trigger an avoidance response. This warrants investigation, since no studies have explored the role of exposure as a contributory mechanism in ABMT. In addition, if exposure proves to be an underpinning mechanism of ABMT, it would be worth exploring different approaches to the training e.g. using inhibition-learning instead of habituation to determine the best way to achieve a longer-term effect of exposure [19].

Conclusion

Testing the proposed mechanistic models of ABMT in AN and investigating their clinical efficacy has the potential of providing a novel treatment approach for AN. It will also contribute to our understanding of cognitive patterns that underlie some of the maladaptive behaviours in AN, e.g. food restriction and fear of food. Lastly, a better understanding of mechanisms and active components of add-on interventions such as ABMT, will help improve treatments for AN.

Availability of data and materials

n/a

Abbreviations

AB:

Attention bias

ABMT:

Attention Bias Modification Training

AN:

Anorexia Nervosa

BED:

Binge Eating Disorder

ED:

Eating disorder

HC:

Healthy controls

References

  1. Brooks S, et al. A systematic review and meta-analysis of cognitive bias to food stimuli in people with disordered eating behaviour. Clin Psychol Rev. 2011;31(1):37–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Giel KE, et al. Attentional processing of food pictures in individuals with anorexia nervosa—an eye-tracking study. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69(7):661–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Werthmann J, et al. Same, same but different: attention bias for food cues in adults and adolescents with anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord. 2019;52(6):681–90.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Mathews A, MacLeod C. Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2005;1:167–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Kakoschke N, Kemps E, Tiggemann M. Attentional bias modification encourages healthy eating. Eat Behav. 2014;15(1):120–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Werthmann J, et al. Looking at food in sad mood: do attention biases lead emotional eaters into overeating after a negative mood induction? Eat Behav. 2014;15(2):230–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Werthmann J, Jansen A, Roefs A. Worry or craving? A selective review of evidence for food-related attention biases in obese individuals, eating-disorder patients, restrained eaters and healthy samples. Proc Nutr Soc. 2015;74(2):99–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. MacLeod C, et al. Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002;111(1):107–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Renwick B, Campbell IC, Schmidt U. Review of Attentional Bias modification: a brain-directed treatment for eating disorders. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2013;21(6):464–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. MacLeod C, Mathews A, Tata P. Attentional bias in emotional disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 1986;95(1):15–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Heeren A, et al. Does attention bias modification improve attentional control? A double-blind randomized experiment with individuals with social anxiety disorder. J Anxiety Disord. 2015;29:35–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Hendrikse JJ, et al. Attentional biases for food cues in overweight and individuals with obesity: a systematic review of the literature. Obes Rev. 2015;16(5):424–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Fodor L-A, Cosmoiu A, Podina I. Cognitive bias modification interventions for attention to and approach of appetitive food stimuli: a meta-analysis. J Evid-Based Psychotherapies. 2017;17:85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Turton R, et al. Novel methods to help develop healthier eating habits for eating and weight disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;61:132–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Schonberg T, et al. Changing value through cued approach: an automatic mechanism of behavior change. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17(4):625–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Taylor CT, et al. Neural correlates of a computerized attention modification program in anxious subjects. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2014;9(9):1379–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Eldar S, Bar-Haim Y. Neural plasticity in response to attention training in anxiety. Psychol Med. 2009;40(4):667–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Goetz PW, Robinson MD, Meier BP. Attentional training of the appetitive motivation system: effects on sensation seeking preferences and reward-based behavior. Motiv Emot. 2008;32(2):120–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Craske MG, et al. Maximizing exposure therapy: an inhibitory learning approach. Behav Res Ther. 2014;58:10–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

n/a

Ethical approval and consent to participate

n/a

Funding

This work was supported by a NARSAD Young Investigator award (25269) from the Brain & Behavior Research Foundation awarded to Jessica Werthmann. Daniela Mercado is financially supported by the Nacional Commission of Science and Technology (CONACYT) awarded by the Mexican government. Iain Campbell, and Ulrike Schmidt receive salary support from the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health, South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London. Ulrike Schmidt is supported by an NIHR Senior Investigator award. The views expressed herein are not those of NIHR or the NHS.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

DM, JW, OO, IC and US were involved in the paper conception. DM drafted the manuscript and JW, OO, IC and US reviewed the paper and informed subsequent drafts. DM, JW, OO, IC and US approved the final protocol paper. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daniela Mercado.

Ethics declarations

Consent for publication

n/a

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mercado, D., Schmidt, U., O’Daly, O.G. et al. Food related attention bias modification training for anorexia nervosa and its potential underpinning mechanisms. J Eat Disord 8, 1 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-019-0276-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-019-0276-9

Keywords